Okay. Is this working? Is this working. On the back? Okay. The task in this week's lectures is try and help you understand a little bit about what comes into the brain and what goes down. We're going to spend a lot of time in discussing what goes on between those two stages, but the access to the outside world and how we affect our muscles are the two primary functions of the brain. Yohannes Mueller was one of the parents of sensory science, along with Helmholtz in the late 19th century, and it's hard to put it better than this. So I'll just read this out from his book in 1935, where he detailed a lot of specific nerve energies. What Miller said was that the same cause, such as electricity, can simultaneously affect all sensory organs, since they are all sensitive to it, and yet every sensory nerve reacts to it differently. One never passes as light, another hears it a sound, another smells it, another tastes. The electricity, another one feels it as pain and shock. One nerve perceives a luminous feature through mechanical irritation. Another one hears it as buzzing. Another one senses it as pain. Sensation is not the conduction of a quality or state of external body to consciousness or the conduction of quality or state of our nerves to consciousness. Excited by an external cause. So in this lovely, flowery 19th century prose that we don't use, unfortunately now what we were trying to say is that we do not have access to the outside world. What we have access to in terms of our perceptions, our cognition is the activity of the sensory nerve fibres that sense the outside world and provide the signals to the rest of the brain. It seems rather commonplace now, but at the time it was quite a revolutionary idea. It has analogies in more modern sensory science, and when we would talk about things called labelled lines, where we think that individual nerve cells contribute to a particular quality or sensation, for example, one nerve cell might signal the readiness of something in the world, another one might signal the greenness of something. Another one might signal the fact that that object appeared in a particular part of your visual field or on a particular part of your skin. Those are the labels that are attached to the activity of those nerve cells. One of the major challenges of neuroscience is to understand how the activity of nerve cells is translated into perceptual and cognitive states. And we are not there yet. But what we do know a lot about is how it is that those nerve cells can provide the signals that we need to access information about the outside world. I can spend several lectures talking to you about the structure of sensory nerve cells and cells, but I'm not. And I'm going to try and instead try and communicate to you three general principles, which I think for me at least, are the basis for understanding sensation. The first is that sensory receptors and we will understand a bit more about sensory receptors in the moment and not evenly distributed. Different parts of the body have different densities of sensory receptors, and for that reason, we use different parts of our bodies for different things. For example, we touch stuff with our fingers. We have a high density of contraceptives. We look at things in particular ways. We trying to bring their gaze, our gaze on objects so that the objects are projected onto the centre of our visual field. There are more photoreceptors in the centre of our visual field, so these different densities of receptors have large implications for how the brain is structured. And I'll take you through that. The second principle is that sensory signals are sent to the cortex along parallel pathways. This doesn't have to be the case. We can imagine a sensory receptor trying to encode everything it can about the outside world and sending all those signals to the rest of the brain. Instead, it seems that some receptors encode something the readiness or the grain issues and others and other things the blueish ness or the green regions of the brain of the outside world. These signals are therefore sent along parallel pathways to the rest of the brain. And this parallelism is the idea that different parts of the outside world are represented within the same modality by different nerve cells is key to understanding how the signals get the cortex. And may also be key to understanding how the cortex is organised. And the third thing I really want to get through in this lecture is that cortex creates the cerebral cortex creates topographic maps of the sensory periphery. I hope you understand the sentence in detail by the time we get through this next 15 minutes. The idea is that the cortical represents representations that we have that we use to see, to feel, to hear. They are constructed representations of the outside world such that the map of the body or the visual field is projected onto the cerebral cortex. And these topographic maps are key to understanding how at least early parts of the cerebral cortex, the initial stages of perception are organised. So these are the three things I really want to try and get through to you in the next 50 minutes. As I said, I could spend five or six lectures on sensory receptors themselves. I'm not going to spend one slide. This is because the basic structure of sensory receptors is pretty similar, and there's only one thing you really need to know about it. That is that those sensory receptors take some form of what we want to say. Interruption of the sensory surface and convert that into a nerve nerve signal, a spiking action potential. For example, photons of light come through in the eye and hit the back of the retina where they sign photoreceptors. And those photoreceptors in turn, transduced that light into an electrochemical energy, which they then pass on to the rest of the brain touch receptors and specialised nerve endings which are sensitive to the displacement of the membrane. So when there's a pressure on to the skin that membrane displaces. And that in turn is converted into electrochemical energy and sent to the rest of the brain ordering auditory receptors. Hence those in the ear specialised receptors which in which those things sense the vibration of the membrane, the tympanic membrane, and they then transform that vibration into magical electrochemical energy that is sent to the rest of the brain. So these sensory nerve endings are all just simply basically transmuting that external stimulus into something that is an action potential. Effectively. I've learned a lot in that sentence, but it's what you need to think about. There's a couple of definitions I just want to get us through as well. The first is that essentially receptors signals the presence, that is the actual detects the presence of an object and signals at a location on the body. So if we can imagine a stimulus saying, let's just think the easiest to think of touching your skin, if you touch it very lightly, you can't feel it. Or you can because if someone else touches in very lightly, you would feel that is only when you make a strong enough indentation of the skin that you can feel pressure. And so as we discussed in a couple of lectures ago, now, those have thresholds. They need a minimum intensity of a stimulus to generate an action potential. And so you can imagine gradually increasing the amount of pressure that someone applies to your skin. And at some point, you will notice that. And that's because as the stimulus intensity increases, so there are small changes in the resting membrane potential of these nerve cells. And at some point in time, that small change is sufficient to drive the occurrence of an action potential. As we talked about in the third or fourth lecture. So there's a threshold, there's a minimum intensity which are below which you're not sensitive. INVISION Minimum intensity is several photons. In a perfectly black environment. We turn all life off here. We did what we call dark adaptation that is sat in the dark for half an hour. And your world photoreceptors incredibly sensitive. They can actually detect the presence of a few photons is really useful if you're running around the savanna late at night trying to avoid lions. And so they can be very sensitive, these receptors, but they still need a minimum level before they can sense something and signal something that would be, say, the threshold. The next point is that as you increase the intensity, the stimulus, at some point in time, you generate a number of action potentials. So at this point in time below here, below the stimulus intensity, this neurone may not be signalling the presence of the stimulus above that it does signal the presence of the stimulus and indeed the action potentials that the neurone produces increase with the intensity of the stimulus. So the number of action potentials is neurone produces signal something about the intensity of the stimulus in the outside world. This function is often called a sigmoid because it looks a bit like an S, and you'll be encountering it several times over the next few weeks. The second related concept is that all sensory neurones have receptive fields. While sometimes we try and work this out, but it's really, really simple. The idea is that sensory stimuli, even if it's an effective stimulus, let's just say it's a finger press on your arm. Each individual nerve cell is sensitive to a particular location on the arm that that finger is put. And if the finger is put somewhere else and the rest of the body at nerve cell is not going to sense it. So it has a receptive field. It has an area of the skin within which this object, your finger, needs to be placed for the pressure to elicit a response from that nerve so that its receptive field will go through that in the second. The same concept can be thought of in terms of vision. In that case, nerve cells in a visual pathway may only respond if a light stimulus is placed in a certain part of the visual field, say up here and to the right. It won't respond if in space over here or down here or up here is the location in the visual field or exactly equivalently the location on the retina where that image is projected, where those receptive fields are. So that's a receptive field location on your body, whether on your body sensory surface, whether it is in the eye, the cochlea, wherever, whereabouts. So it takes information from. It only takes you from a limited part of the visual world, from limb, part of the body, etc.. And you can think of this. We often think of these receptive fields as having a non-uniform sensitivity across that, and we'll get into that in a second. But the idea here is that basically at the periphery of the recipe field, the nerve cell is not very sensitive. You have to make very strong indentations for the nerve cell to respond, whereas in the centre of the receptive field, it's very sensitive in response to slightly weaker indentations. We can measure that by, for example, placing a stimulus at different points relative to the receptive field. And if we do that and measure the number of spikes that are produced by a neurone, we would see something like this profile, like a Gaussian, usually a normal distribution whereby the same stimulus is capable of finding many spikes when space in the centre of the receptive field, but only a few in space on the periphery. Now, the observant of you there will think, well, hang on a sec. If if there is any field means that the number of spikes in your produces for the same stimulus depends on the location with respect to this centre of the receptive field. And the number of spikes produced depends on the intensive stimulus. Isn't there a confound there? Isn't there something? Couldn't I trade off the intensity of stimulus for the spatial position and get the same number of action cancels from this nerve? So. And if you were thinking that that's precisely correct, that's one of the major compounds in sensory pathways, trying to extract the important things from what is often multiple different types of causes that could give rise to the same number of action potentials. And hopefully we'll be able to get through that a little bit later in this lecture. So I said that receptive fields at his low clients, discreet places in your skin or in your eyes or wherever with something is responsive to this is most nicely exhibited in the snout essentially system because the body the skin provides his surface, which he can explicitly think about or simply feels. And there's actually been some recordings from humans here where you put a small electrode into one of the nerves in the arm and you can, in five circumstances, pick up the activity of sensory nerve cells and those nerves. And you can map out the respective fields of both sensory nerve cells. And it turns out that they look a little bit like this, honey, to form circles on the hand here. And you see that they have different sizes such that they're a bit larger. If you're on the palm of the hand and it's quite a bit smaller from the fingertip. And you can think of that here. For example, having many small receptive fields on the thumb of the finger and large ones on the palm of the arm. Fewer of them. So it turns out this is, as I said, as a general principle, since we systems that distribution of receptors on the body or in the eye is not the same across the whole body. Some parts of the body have much higher density of receptors and some have much lower density. However, the whole surface is what we call tile or antisense receptors. That is, every part of the skin or every part of the eye has at least one sense for detecting stuff in there. So the combination of these two things, the timing and the sizes, has profound consequences on our sensory abilities. You can actually do this yourself, or rather you can do this with a partner. If you get a paper clip and expose the two ends, you can do what's called a two point discrimination. The idea there is that if you move the ends of the pits further and closer together, you can change the distance on the other parts of the skin that you're going to stimulate when you press that paperclip onto the skin. Now, if those two points are close enough together and you put it on, say your arm here, you detect, you experience the sensation of just having a single pinprick on your on your arm. If, on the other hand, you stuck that on the finger, you will detect or experience two different distinct pinpricks in your fingertip. So what's the reason for that? Why is it that the same stimulus feels like one thing? One's on your arm or on your palm for two things. One is on your fingertips. So the reason for that can be is logical from the structure of these receptive fields on your fingertips or on your thumb. The receptive fields are very small, so that seems distance apart of those two pinpricks will activate two distinct sensory receptors and evacuating two distinct sensory receptors, you will sense two distinct objects that are touching your skin. On the other hand, if those same interests are made on your arm, you're activating only a single sensory receptor. And if you activate only a single sensory receptor, you'll feel only one object on your skin. So the size of these receptors means that you can distinguish between two objects or one object being present. I'm not going to talk much about the subcortical pathways for presentation. The key point here in the Spanish sensory system, and as we'll discover in a second in the visual system, is that these signals are taken from the skin, in this case through the spinal cord, taken up through the thalamus, everything goes through the thalamus of cerebral cortex, and they project upon what is called the primary somatic sensory cortex. It's not a sensory to touch primary because that's the major source of input from the thalamus. So the primary cortices are those part of the cerebral cortex, the input from the thalamic relay cells for each different modality. And they form this, they project in this case onto the present. So we'll see in a second in the cerebral cortex. By the way, a lot of what we know about this is actually from work, from Walter Penfield and his colleagues in McGill. Back in the 1940s and fifties, where as they were preparing patients for surgery for epilepsy, actually made in different parts of the cerebral cortex. And that's what the patients felt. These experiments are no longer very often conducted, but they were incredibly illuminating at the time. They were able to map out in humans the structure of this amount of sensory cortex, for example, by asking people what they felt when you stimulated different parts of the cortex. So this is the central stool because these are going to be the potential supertankers get some fees. This is a central focus as part of the major Suvi in the brain and before pre and post after the simple superset is post mains behind towards the back of the brain premiums in front was a frontal brain and if you looked along this potential suitcase, you find a structure which is incredibly beautiful. What happens there is that if you stimulate different parts of this personal sulcus, some person will report that they feel sensations at different parts of their body. So, for example, if you down on the lateral side and you stimulate it, you might find that some reports that they felt a sensation on their face. Whereas if you're up on the top medial side, that report, instead of sensation on that front or on the foot or leg. So depending on whereabouts alone, this person was suicidal whereabouts and cerebral sucres are you are encoding. You are representing activity on different parts of your skin. I think this gives rise to the concept, which is the homunculus in terms of the touch among us means little man. And if you look at the representation of the body on this first episode, the touch representation, you find that this continuous representation of different parts of the body and different parts of the body that are closer together are generally speaking, represented closer together on the cortex. So if example, the foot is represented a similar location to the leg or the trunk, whereas if face the lips and the nose are represented close to each other and in between these of that hand in the arm. So you get this map of the body that's formed on this matter sensory cortex. It's the different types of touch that you can get in different parts of your body as represented on the similar sensory cortex. And you find that actually some parts of the body seem to be overrepresented. For example, the face is a large part of the sensory cortex, whereas the foot is quite a small part, even though its size is relatively speaking, even not in many cases in the face or the trunk, for example, occupies almost a minuscule part of the cerebral cortex. And the reason for that is fairly obvious if you think of just one simple principle. Every sensory receptor has about the same amount of cerebral cortex devoted to it. It follows then that we have more sensor receptors, for example, on your finger, on your thumb, on your face, you'll have more cerebral cortex devoted to that part of the body. And when you have less sensory receptors, it shrunk your arm, your leg, you have less part of the body devoted to that is what we would call cortical magnification. The idea that the cerebral cortex is like a magnifying glass onto your body. That magnifying glass, how much it magnifies depend on the density of the sensory receptors at that part of your body. This is what the locomotive looks like if you're trying to represent it as an intact human being. This cortical magnification has some implications. It means that your ability, as I said, to detect small changes in the position of objects or the presence of two objects instead of one depends on the density of sensory receptors and therefore the amount of cerebral cortex that's actually devoted to that part of the skin. This graph here shows you compares is perceptual, the psychophysical acuity that you have the different parts of your body and that low numbers in mean high acuity. That means you're very sensitive to small distances and large numbers being low acuity. That means you in much larger distances between objects to determine tunes at one there and can see that the areas of the body with the largest or the lowest acuity at a lower arm of the arm, shoulder, belly, back breast, thigh, half all those areas where you know yourself that when you touch those things, you're very less tense, very much less sensitive to the structure of the things that are touching that part of the body. Whereas for example, the fingers and the upper lip, the cheek, the nose have much higher acuity and much more sensitive to different the structure of the things that are touching the body. So this density of receptors determines the amount of cerebral cortex that is actually devoted to that part of the sensory apparatus. And that amount of cortex is devoted to that. Sensory in turn dictates how sensitive or how how much acuity you have, the sensory stimuli that impinge in that part of the body. I just briefly wanted to show you the structure, the visual pathways, very similar. I'm not going to go through all these lines. I just want to illustrate to you from a paper that we produced many years ago now, that in the eye there are retinal nerve cells which include photoreceptors that signal they're actually communicated by a little network of cells in the retina, which you learn about later stages via ganglion cells, whose axons make up the optic nerve. Those ganglion cells in turn go to this little structure in the thalamus the lateral clinic is, and from there their signals projected the primary visual cortex or V1. So very similar in structure to this, not a sensory pathway, except that one went through the spinal cord to get the thalamus and then the cortex or this one from the eye or straight through the optic nerve, through the thalamus and the visual cortex. There are different pathways from the eye to the thalamus, and that's to visual cortex. We often call these the P of the M or the PARTICELLE, and the Minnesota pathways is quite a bit in the reading that I suggested you do that discusses how the signals of these nerve cells differ. I just want to introduce you to the idea that they actually have different structure and different types of signals. Now, I said on the cement essentially surface, you can tell quite easily that the finger, for example, is what you used to touch on things, and you can feel the fine gradations in the texture, for example, the surface. Similarly, if you look in the eye, if you take a photo through the eye and this is what a photo through the eye looks like, you see an object. This is the bit where the optic nerve starts with the axons of the ganglion cells come out of the eye and go into the optic nerve. It's also the place where the blood vessels come into the eye from the optic nerve. This is the picture of the eye through a fan scope and in the middle of the eye. See, the structure is called the phobia. It's an incredible structure. And this structure, you've got no blood vessels. This is Photoshopped as a smaller than anywhere else in the body. And then the other apparatus in the retina has been pushed away. So the photos have to have direct access to the light that comes through the lens and hits the retina. And in this location, in this part of the eye, the small part of the eye is about three or four millimetres in size. You have this incredibly dense population of nerve cells called cone photoreceptors, and that's represented by this paper down here. You can see the density of these cones peaks in that area. And that means that there's because there's so many different, so many more photoreceptors in this particular part of the eye that you can distinguish between an object that's slightly displaced. So when I wanted to see the structure of the visual world, what I need to do is I need to move my eyes around so that part of the world that I'm interested in falls on the phobia. Because then I can distinguish the difference between what might be, for example, a happy face, a sad face, a bald face. So if I want to see that fine special detail, someone at the back of the room who's sitting there looking at me, that is about one quarter of my thumb. And is it slightly less than one degree of ice? If I want to be able to distinguish the difference between someone's eyes or their face to recognise their face, I have to bring my phobia onto that object. I have to move my eyes so that that part of the visual falls onto my phobia where I have this really dense array of photoreceptors. I won't talk about them at all. But the gods, the ones which are important for night vision actually more dense just outside of the phobia. And so it turns out if you're ever out there in the dark night looking at the stars, if you want to see a star, you don't look directly at it, you slightly side of it. And that's because of what photoreceptors are actually absent from the photograph from the centre of your gaze. And instead you need to bring that light onto the side of your phobia with regard to actually most dense. It turns out that this structure, this density photo receptors in the in the phobia, which is so much greater in the phobia than elsewhere in the eye, is paralleled by changes in the structure of the subsequent nerve cells in the eye. And the consequence is that we can't see fine spatial detail in the periphery of our vision. This again is the idea of cortical magnification. We can see fine spatial detail when we're looking directly at something, but not when away from that centre of our guys. Our cortex has magnified that small part of the visual field, which is occupied by the phobia. I think that's about the size of your thumb. That part of the visual field where all these thousands of kind photoreceptors are sitting waiting for light to come. The primary visual cortex is actually totally magnifying that part of the visual field. Some estimates would put it to be something like 20 or 30% of primary visual cortex is devoted to this tiny little part of the visual field, and the rest of the visual field is consequently represented by many fewer ganglion cells, cells in the cortex. So therefore we're much less capable of seeing the finer spatial detail away from the centre of gaze because the cortical magnification is so pronounced for us. For real. You can see this yourself. If you have a look at this slight demonstration here. If you look in the centre of this thing here on the projector, you should be able to read. If you look where the arrow is, you should be able to define or distinguish what each of the different letters is. Does everyone agree with that? Approximately. If you look at the centre, you can still see this on the left, the K on the right and left from bottom on the top. It's like going to. I then if, on the other hand, you look at the air over here, you should not be able to define many of the letters. They are present there. So you may be able to see that the ace in the S is there and you could see the K in the R, But many of the things like T.P. y are actually indecipherable to you. And the reason for that is that what I've done in making this diagram here is scale the size of the letters so that they occupy approximately the same amount of cerebral cortex when you're looking at the centre of the diagram. And because they'll find the same amount of cerebral cortex, you're equally able to see all those letters. But when we look at the eye and this is no longer matched, sometimes that is a much smaller and occupying enough of the cerebral cortex. The magnification is wrong. So this viewpoint. So you are less able to be able to detect what these different letters are. Hmm. I said that there were parallel pathways that take the signals from the eye to the visual cortex, and these are quite pronounced if you look in the thalamus. You can see in this little structure overlap, which is a beautiful structure. I spent most of my life studying it. So I think it's beautiful. It's called that unique because it looks a bit like a me. If you're younger than me, actually bend your knee, then you would look a little bit like me. And in this little genic nucleus, which you can see by the naked eye is these different layers or parts of magnet. So it layers smaller and larger cell bodies. And it is these layers. Now we know that these cells would communicate different things to the cerebral cortex. These different layers get different inputs from the eye, in particular these ganglion cells, the cells that form apple to the eye, to the thalamus. The particular ones are much smaller. They're going to pass through the magnet. So everyone's a much larger. They're going to be sterilised. And then these signals of these thalamic neurones then go to primary visual cortex. Now, turns out there's very few ways to test this. But the only in fact, the only way to really test is supply small lesions, as we discussed last week. You can place small lesions in the brain, in animals, in a small controlled lesions, and you can destroy some of the nerve cells in the palm. So all the magnet, some of the layers. And if you try an animal farm to report things about the outside world and therefore ask whether or not these nerve cells that come from the right things in the retina and send them to the cerebral cortex with these nerve cells saying different things, the cerebral cortex. And it turns out they do. So, for example, in this set of beautiful work is Bill Murray and to conduct in the late 1980s and 90. Summarised in this review that I cited here, the idea is that there's a lesion this place in the palm of the layers of the macaque monkey. Or in the magnesium layers of the of a different macaque monkey with monkey before the length made. This monkey is trained to report simple things about the outside world. What the colour they trying to simply report. What is something they will not. In a particular location. Hmm. If there was something that I went to make one important. If there wasn't a to make another report. These graphs here, the solid lines show the capacity of the animal to detect something which varies either in the kind of striping this of the patterns that are present or in the flicker. That is the kind of amount of times per second something flickers, a light flickers. Africa is defined by the tempo frequency. That is how many times a second something, because the spatial frequency here is just how many of these fine bars you have in one degree of visual angle, the solid limestone, what the monkey does in the normal case without a lesion and the different points. So the monkeys performance when you've after you've made a lesion, it turns out that if you make a lesion at the end pathway, you have almost no impact on the monkey's capacity to detect the spatial form of an object. Whereas if you make a leap into the pathway, this is almost most. On the other hand, if you make a lesion at the pathway you detect, you abolish the animal's capacities, the black lines here to detect very rapidly flickering things. Whereas if you abolish the pathway, we preserve the capacity to detect those rapidly flickering things. And finally, if you lesion the pathway, you kind of the monkey cannot see coloured objects, whereas if you lesion the pathway, the monkey can. So these things, these different perceptual abilities in the presence and absence of different pathways suggest that the different signals that come from the eye to the cortex carry qualitatively different and qualitatively different signals about the outside world that carried along the parallel pathways to the cerebral cortex where they are. Then we combine. So when we get to Cortex, we've got all these parallel pathways doing stuff in the sensory periphery, whether in visual cortex and other sensory cortex, wherever it is. We have these parallel pathways from a sensory final thalamus, bringing all this information up to the cortex. And somehow the cortex has to rearrange these interesting judgements about the outside world. For many years. Before that, the brain was basically you what you're born with. You had that there was no plasticity in the brain. It took several decades of experiments to actually reject that hypothesis. I want to show you a couple of those experiments in the substance lines on this line. These are some of the very early experiments that were able to reject the hypothesis that the brain, the cerebral cortex in particular, was indifferent to experience. It was the same when you were born as when you were dying. These two experiments relied again on work on monkeys because these monkeys were easy enough to be trained to report what had happened to to the outside world. In the first experiment here, I want to say is the monkey was trained to hold the fingers against a little rotating disc, and I had to make judgements about that rotating this, I think the direction of motion, that rotating disk using the fingers only. The question was, would the experience, the long term experience of making this judgement with your fingers change the representation of the fingers in the cerebral cortex? And the way that the researchers went about trying to address that question is that they made electrophysiological recordings from this ninth century cortex of these monkeys before, during and after training to do this simple task. And what they find is summarised in this slide here, which is a little bit complicated, but the end result is very straightforward. So this is recording from this mid-century area of an al monkey, people that monkey, and they're recording from the region that is most important in representing the hand associated before it's a monkey. Listen, the sensory cortex that's there now monkeys as it is in humans. What they did was they made recordings from these part of the cortex before experience, and they found a particular representation of the digits, the final four digits, five, which of the hand in this part of the cortex. And that's described over here. So is the fifth, fourth, second or third digit. And the normal here. In the normal case, these different digits have approximately equal parts of the cortex devoted to them. However, following this experience, following this training, you find a substantial overrepresentation of the second and the third digit, and those are the two digit, the two fingers that the animal is using to make this judgement. So prolonged exposure to these kind of tasks has changed how the cerebral cortex is organised. It has increased the amount of cerebral cortex that's important, that's used for extending information from those two digits. The animals used to do the task. So the brain is plastic. The organisation, the cerebral cortex is plastic. It can adapt to the structure of experience and tasks that we would need to accomplish. Although a substantial amount of work as shown this many different systems since, and this was the original work to show that this plasticity was there. I really encourage you to read it with a beautiful set of experiments. The converse is it can also be studied. That is what happens when you lose a digit or lose some part of your sensory periphery. In this case, if the animal, for example, loses a third digit. This case is surgically removed and the seizure. Again, you can make the recordings before and after that surgery. And again, you can measure in this case again from one case in the parts of the brain that represent the hand. And you find, at least in some cases and this is still controversial, that when the third digit is removed, a bit of the cerebral cortex that was responsible for including things that happened on that anger now include things that happened on the second or the fourth or the ninth and fingers. It's like this piece of cerebral cortex wants to do something, wants to do anything. And in the absence of any input from the third digit, it's asking for input from the second and the fourth digits help the brain represent things that are going on there. So the brain is the cerebral cortex is plastic. It can adapt to changes in the input from the outside world. Say this is not uncontroversial. In some systems, this seems to be less the case, and some systems seem to be more the case. It is certainly dependent on what kind of life injury happened early. Younger people who suffer injuries have more cortical plasticity. Older people have less. One of the things that this leads to is phantom limb. And I just want to spend a couple of seconds showing you a really effective video from one of the leaders in this field. Ramachandran. I find this quite an effective video, so just spend a couple minutes on. Twins First patients this Derek Steen. All right. One of Ramachandran first patients was Derek Steen. 13 years ago, he was involved in a motorcycle accident, and I pulled the nerves out of my spinal cord up in my neck. They told my parents directly that I would never use my arm again. About seven years ago, I was reading through the classifieds and I saw an ad in there. Amputees wanted that. It was a joke like that. It's just basically connecting the club to the ball. So I called the number and it was Dr. Ramachandran. Go relax. Today, Derek is teaching Ramachandran how to play golf. But several years ago, Derek made a crucial contribution to Ramachandran pioneering work in brain science. Yes, I was amazing. After my surgery, I sat up in the bed and still felt the arm there. Still felt everything there. And I'm looking down and I'm seeing nothing. It was pretty bizarre. The more I thought about it, the more it hurt. The more it hurt, the more I thought about it. So it was it was like it was never ending. I mean, I'd break out in a cold sweat and turn pale. Just standing here talking to you because the pain would hit so bad. If there is any one thing about our existence that we take for granted. It's the fact that we have a body. Each of us has a body. And, you know, you give it a name, it has a bank account and so on and so forth. But it turns out even your body is something that you construct in your mind. And this is what we call your body image. Now, of course, in my case, it's substantiated by the fact that I really use a body with bone and tissue. But the sense I have, the internal sense I have of the presence of a body and arms and all of that is, of course, constructed in my brain and it's in my mind. And the most striking evidence for this comes from these patients who have had an amputation and continue to feel the presence of the missing. How? It was the beginning of an important relationship. Important for Derek, because not only would he finally understand his phantom pain, he would also get to the bottom of a mysterious sensation he felt while shaving. When I first started shaving after my surgery, I would feel my absent hand start to hurt and tingle whenever I shaved this left side of my face. Meeting Derrick was important for Ramachandran because the explanation he came up with would rock the world of neuroscience. Photograph. That's just my arm. The first thing Ramachandran did was to invite Derek to his lab for a simple test that I want to touch different parts of your body. And I just want you to tell me what you feel and where you experience the sensation. Close your eyes. I could feel that on my forehead. Anything anywhere else? No. So my nose. Okay. My chest. Your chest. Okay. I can feel that on my cheek and I can feel rubbing on the phantom left hand. On the phantom left hand in addition to your cheek, I'm going to run the Q-Tip across your jaw and see what happens. I can feel like you did by my cheek and I can feel a stroking sensation across the phantom hand. You actually feel that stroking across your phantom hand. Okay, so that small visual video and you can just it goes on for a while. I encourage you to watch it. That shows the fact that this person has lost their arm, that some part of their representation of their body has distorted not just the inside of the brain, but also perceptually cognitively. And the likely explanation for this is that one part of the likely explanation for this is that the representation of the face is actually quite close to the representation of the hand. And as we saw with the monkey who is missing the third digit, when you lose inputs to certain parts of the cerebral cortex, that cortex seems to want to do something anyway, stop to draw input from neighbouring cortical areas. So that part of the body which was representing the arm is now no longer there. Now he's also drawing input from the face as clearly more complicated than just simply saying that because this person's body image is constructed, image is something that is not simply explained just by the amount of sensory cortex, but that distortion in the cortical representation is going to contribute to the fact that this person feels something, even though there is no longer there is plasticity is important in helping this, in helping the brain effectively try to reconstruct or to do what it would like to do, even in the absence of inputs. I just want to spend a couple of minutes facing what will be spending most of the next lecture on or the next one. And I'd really like you to do some reading in the next section, which is this review that I've put up online from Colby and go back to leaders in the field reviews to look at old. Now that is probably the best conceptualisation of the ideas we'll go through in the next lecture. We discussed that, that sometimes that is kind of a frame of reference in which you understand these sensations is is depends on how you want to think about things. I just want to explain to you what I mean by frames of reference for a few slides. So when we look at the cerebral cortex, we see this translated into many distinct areas and visual cortex, for example, as primary visual cortex. But then there's about three or four, maybe even ten or 15 different visual areas that sit next to primary visual cortex, the whole higher order cortical areas or association cortex. The same is the case in similar sensory cortex, the same effects in the auditory cortex. You have these primary areas, then you have these multiple other satellite areas. And the question arises, one that actually puzzled researchers for many decades now is why do you have so many cortical areas? Why don't we just have one area that's responsible for vision, one area that's responsible for autism? And the hypothesis that I'd like to explore in the next lecture is very much like these parallel pathways from the sensory periphery to cerebral cortex. These different cortical areas act as parallel representations or parallel constructions of the outside world. Each area is doing something, creating a slightly different interpretation of the outside world. This then raises the question of how can these different the things that are arising in these different cortical areas be brought back together? How can the different maps of the outside, both the construction of the outside world, be reconciled? And the second thing that starts when I ask is these topographical photographs and statements, these things that are maps of your body, your maps of your eyes, that's fine If we want to, you know, represent the precise location in our body that something happens. But it's not very useful if we want to move around the world where I need to know where my location is with respect to this table. With respect to the microphone. With respect to these chairs. So the question that arises is how these topographic maps of the sensory body of the body of the eye, how these transformed into something that could be behaviourally useful, could actually help us move around the world accomplish tasks. It was not very useful. Just simply know that this is a place of my hand or my arm. Sorry. I would like to know where that place is with respect. For example, if my arm is moved with respect to the rest of my body. So I want to order frame a frame of reference in which I can understand these different aspects of my movement throughout the world. And that's that is the majority past of what we call the parietal cortex. And that's what we're going to be spending the next half, the next lecture on. And as I said, I'd really like you to read that coding review because that will help you understand what it is that the cortex is trying to do, how it's constructing maps of the outside world that we can use to move around them. And so we, we, we investigated that on Friday, and I look forward to seeing you there. Thanks. Yeah. I was. Yes. So it's and particularly in two volumes. It was. Particularly a good topic for the show. But something about. Are you actually doing. It where it's kind of. Surprising for you to feel particular? I can't quite remember where where we are at with. I think the general sensation of like, I'm going to take on you, that's relatively easy enough to understand. This is the migration and it's kind of been associated with the reaction. You know how it is that. According to what's called a predictive coding framework in the world. Where you can predict quite well what the temptation is that you. Should get. Right because you're doing. Something and you can because you know what you should be getting ready to. Predict and therefore surprise what is predictable. And think there's a framework of understanding brain function, which Professor SEO has been particularly. Important from guidance. Which is that the job of the brain, you basically find out things that are not. And so a lot of the architecture, the brains that Christians are predicting, that includes, you know, perception, suppressing, not encoding things that, you know. I mean, for example, I think that. You can suppress your own sensations, especially during actions. So that's predictive protein, which is really influential in. On the questions regarding the object. Yes. So yes, in exactly the same way. It's easier to see the things overlap altogether. Like, yeah. That's where you can. Something. I think, optimism. And because here. And in my. You. Yeah. Yeah. The overlapping. For one thing, the idea of hiring people sometimes out. I'm very. Part of life, as you can imagine, is just a single. You would. But even in this case. Consequently. That often. That changes. Central brain function. Yes. Like many. And so even when you figure. It was final. Expect to really point and explain everything. Thank you. Thank you. So. And would the to. Yeah, I look at that because it's so much easier to like. Yeah. Okay. Yeah. You know, like in our. I mean, the idea is to. Always have an interactive story, but I don't know the exact exact. I like. I felt. I don't know. I mean. Yes. Okay. And I knew it would be like. Well, this. So people. And. We know. Know. I. You know this. Yeah. Yeah. Oh. Yeah. People. I think. And.