y2clutch / raw_transcripts /lecture_7.txt
elyx's picture
initial commit
dae990d
raw
history blame
48.4 kB
Okay.
Is this working?
Is this working.
On the back?
Okay.
The task in this week's lectures is try and help
you understand a little bit about what comes into the
brain and what goes down.
We're going to spend a lot of time in discussing
what goes on between those two stages, but the access
to the outside world and how we affect our muscles
are the two primary functions of the brain.
Yohannes Mueller was one of the parents of sensory science,
along with Helmholtz in the late 19th century, and it's
hard to put it better than this.
So I'll just read this out from his book in
1935, where he detailed a lot of specific nerve energies.
What Miller said was that the same cause, such as
electricity, can simultaneously affect all sensory organs, since they are
all sensitive to it, and yet every sensory nerve reacts
to it differently.
One never passes as light, another hears it a sound,
another smells it, another tastes.
The electricity, another one feels it as pain and shock.
One nerve perceives a luminous feature through mechanical irritation.
Another one hears it as buzzing.
Another one senses it as pain.
Sensation is not the conduction of a quality or state
of external body to consciousness or the conduction of quality
or state of our nerves to consciousness.
Excited by an external cause.
So in this lovely, flowery 19th century prose that we
don't use, unfortunately now what we were trying to say
is that we do not have access to the outside
world.
What we have access to in terms of our perceptions,
our cognition is the activity of the sensory nerve fibres
that sense the outside world and provide the signals to
the rest of the brain.
It seems rather commonplace now, but at the time it
was quite a revolutionary idea.
It has analogies in more modern sensory science, and when
we would talk about things called labelled lines, where we
think that individual nerve cells contribute to a particular quality
or sensation, for example, one nerve cell might signal the
readiness of something in the world, another one might signal
the greenness of something.
Another one might signal the fact that that object appeared
in a particular part of your visual field or on
a particular part of your skin.
Those are the labels that are attached to the activity
of those nerve cells.
One of the major challenges of neuroscience is to understand
how the activity of nerve cells is translated into perceptual
and cognitive states.
And we are not there yet.
But what we do know a lot about is how
it is that those nerve cells can provide the signals
that we need to access information about the outside world.
I can spend several lectures talking to you about the
structure of sensory nerve cells and cells, but I'm not.
And I'm going to try and instead try and communicate
to you three general principles, which I think for me
at least, are the basis for understanding sensation.
The first is that sensory receptors and we will understand
a bit more about sensory receptors in the moment and
not evenly distributed.
Different parts of the body have different densities of sensory
receptors, and for that reason, we use different parts of
our bodies for different things.
For example, we touch stuff with our fingers.
We have a high density of contraceptives.
We look at things in particular ways.
We trying to bring their gaze, our gaze on objects
so that the objects are projected onto the centre of
our visual field.
There are more photoreceptors in the centre of our visual
field, so these different densities of receptors have large implications
for how the brain is structured.
And I'll take you through that.
The second principle is that sensory signals are sent to
the cortex along parallel pathways.
This doesn't have to be the case.
We can imagine a sensory receptor trying to encode everything
it can about the outside world and sending all those
signals to the rest of the brain.
Instead, it seems that some receptors encode something the readiness
or the grain issues and others and other things the
blueish ness or the green regions of the brain of
the outside world.
These signals are therefore sent along parallel pathways to the
rest of the brain.
And this parallelism is the idea that different parts of
the outside world are represented within the same modality by
different nerve cells is key to understanding how the signals
get the cortex.
And may also be key to understanding how the cortex
is organised.
And the third thing I really want to get through
in this lecture is that cortex creates the cerebral cortex
creates topographic maps of the sensory periphery.
I hope you understand the sentence in detail by the
time we get through this next 15 minutes.
The idea is that the cortical represents representations that we
have that we use to see, to feel, to hear.
They are constructed representations of the outside world such that
the map of the body or the visual field is
projected onto the cerebral cortex.
And these topographic maps are key to understanding how at
least early parts of the cerebral cortex, the initial stages
of perception are organised.
So these are the three things I really want to
try and get through to you in the next 50
minutes.
As I said, I could spend five or six lectures
on sensory receptors themselves.
I'm not going to spend one slide.
This is because the basic structure of sensory receptors is
pretty similar, and there's only one thing you really need
to know about it.
That is that those sensory receptors take some form of
what we want to say.
Interruption of the sensory surface and convert that into a
nerve nerve signal, a spiking action potential.
For example, photons of light come through in the eye
and hit the back of the retina where they sign
photoreceptors.
And those photoreceptors in turn, transduced that light into an
electrochemical energy, which they then pass on to the rest
of the brain touch receptors and specialised nerve endings which
are sensitive to the displacement of the membrane.
So when there's a pressure on to the skin that
membrane displaces.
And that in turn is converted into electrochemical energy and
sent to the rest of the brain ordering auditory receptors.
Hence those in the ear specialised receptors which in which
those things sense the vibration of the membrane, the tympanic
membrane, and they then transform that vibration into magical electrochemical
energy that is sent to the rest of the brain.
So these sensory nerve endings are all just simply basically
transmuting that external stimulus into something that is an action
potential.
Effectively.
I've learned a lot in that sentence, but it's what
you need to think about.
There's a couple of definitions I just want to get
us through as well.
The first is that essentially receptors signals the presence, that
is the actual detects the presence of an object and
signals at a location on the body.
So if we can imagine a stimulus saying, let's just
think the easiest to think of touching your skin, if
you touch it very lightly, you can't feel it.
Or you can because if someone else touches in very
lightly, you would feel that is only when you make
a strong enough indentation of the skin that you can
feel pressure.
And so as we discussed in a couple of lectures
ago, now, those have thresholds.
They need a minimum intensity of a stimulus to generate
an action potential.
And so you can imagine gradually increasing the amount of
pressure that someone applies to your skin.
And at some point, you will notice that.
And that's because as the stimulus intensity increases, so there
are small changes in the resting membrane potential of these
nerve cells.
And at some point in time, that small change is
sufficient to drive the occurrence of an action potential.
As we talked about in the third or fourth lecture.
So there's a threshold, there's a minimum intensity which are
below which you're not sensitive.
INVISION Minimum intensity is several photons.
In a perfectly black environment.
We turn all life off here.
We did what we call dark adaptation that is sat
in the dark for half an hour.
And your world photoreceptors incredibly sensitive.
They can actually detect the presence of a few photons
is really useful if you're running around the savanna late
at night trying to avoid lions.
And so they can be very sensitive, these receptors, but
they still need a minimum level before they can sense
something and signal something that would be, say, the threshold.
The next point is that as you increase the intensity,
the stimulus, at some point in time, you generate a
number of action potentials.
So at this point in time below here, below the
stimulus intensity, this neurone may not be signalling the presence
of the stimulus above that it does signal the presence
of the stimulus and indeed the action potentials that the
neurone produces increase with the intensity of the stimulus.
So the number of action potentials is neurone produces signal
something about the intensity of the stimulus in the outside
world.
This function is often called a sigmoid because it looks
a bit like an S, and you'll be encountering it
several times over the next few weeks.
The second related concept is that all sensory neurones have
receptive fields.
While sometimes we try and work this out, but it's
really, really simple.
The idea is that sensory stimuli, even if it's an
effective stimulus, let's just say it's a finger press on
your arm.
Each individual nerve cell is sensitive to a particular location
on the arm that that finger is put.
And if the finger is put somewhere else and the
rest of the body at nerve cell is not going
to sense it.
So it has a receptive field.
It has an area of the skin within which this
object, your finger, needs to be placed for the pressure
to elicit a response from that nerve so that its
receptive field will go through that in the second.
The same concept can be thought of in terms of
vision.
In that case, nerve cells in a visual pathway may
only respond if a light stimulus is placed in a
certain part of the visual field, say up here and
to the right.
It won't respond if in space over here or down
here or up here is the location in the visual
field or exactly equivalently the location on the retina where
that image is projected, where those receptive fields are.
So that's a receptive field location on your body, whether
on your body sensory surface, whether it is in the
eye, the cochlea, wherever, whereabouts.
So it takes information from.
It only takes you from a limited part of the
visual world, from limb, part of the body, etc..
And you can think of this.
We often think of these receptive fields as having a
non-uniform sensitivity across that, and we'll get into that in
a second.
But the idea here is that basically at the periphery
of the recipe field, the nerve cell is not very
sensitive.
You have to make very strong indentations for the nerve
cell to respond, whereas in the centre of the receptive
field, it's very sensitive in response to slightly weaker indentations.
We can measure that by, for example, placing a stimulus
at different points relative to the receptive field.
And if we do that and measure the number of
spikes that are produced by a neurone, we would see
something like this profile, like a Gaussian, usually a normal
distribution whereby the same stimulus is capable of finding many
spikes when space in the centre of the receptive field,
but only a few in space on the periphery.
Now, the observant of you there will think, well, hang
on a sec.
If if there is any field means that the number
of spikes in your produces for the same stimulus depends
on the location with respect to this centre of the
receptive field.
And the number of spikes produced depends on the intensive
stimulus.
Isn't there a confound there?
Isn't there something?
Couldn't I trade off the intensity of stimulus for the
spatial position and get the same number of action cancels
from this nerve?
So.
And if you were thinking that that's precisely correct, that's
one of the major compounds in sensory pathways, trying to
extract the important things from what is often multiple different
types of causes that could give rise to the same
number of action potentials.
And hopefully we'll be able to get through that a
little bit later in this lecture.
So I said that receptive fields at his low clients,
discreet places in your skin or in your eyes or
wherever with something is responsive to this is most nicely
exhibited in the snout essentially system because the body the
skin provides his surface, which he can explicitly think about
or simply feels.
And there's actually been some recordings from humans here where
you put a small electrode into one of the nerves
in the arm and you can, in five circumstances, pick
up the activity of sensory nerve cells and those nerves.
And you can map out the respective fields of both
sensory nerve cells.
And it turns out that they look a little bit
like this, honey, to form circles on the hand here.
And you see that they have different sizes such that
they're a bit larger.
If you're on the palm of the hand and it's
quite a bit smaller from the fingertip.
And you can think of that here.
For example, having many small receptive fields on the thumb
of the finger and large ones on the palm of
the arm.
Fewer of them.
So it turns out this is, as I said, as
a general principle, since we systems that distribution of receptors
on the body or in the eye is not the
same across the whole body.
Some parts of the body have much higher density of
receptors and some have much lower density.
However, the whole surface is what we call tile or
antisense receptors.
That is, every part of the skin or every part
of the eye has at least one sense for detecting
stuff in there.
So the combination of these two things, the timing and
the sizes, has profound consequences on our sensory abilities.
You can actually do this yourself, or rather you can
do this with a partner.
If you get a paper clip and expose the two
ends, you can do what's called a two point discrimination.
The idea there is that if you move the ends
of the pits further and closer together, you can change
the distance on the other parts of the skin that
you're going to stimulate when you press that paperclip onto
the skin.
Now, if those two points are close enough together and
you put it on, say your arm here, you detect,
you experience the sensation of just having a single pinprick
on your on your arm.
If, on the other hand, you stuck that on the
finger, you will detect or experience two different distinct pinpricks
in your fingertip.
So what's the reason for that?
Why is it that the same stimulus feels like one
thing?
One's on your arm or on your palm for two
things.
One is on your fingertips.
So the reason for that can be is logical from
the structure of these receptive fields on your fingertips or
on your thumb.
The receptive fields are very small, so that seems distance
apart of those two pinpricks will activate two distinct sensory
receptors and evacuating two distinct sensory receptors, you will sense
two distinct objects that are touching your skin.
On the other hand, if those same interests are made
on your arm, you're activating only a single sensory receptor.
And if you activate only a single sensory receptor, you'll
feel only one object on your skin.
So the size of these receptors means that you can
distinguish between two objects or one object being present.
I'm not going to talk much about the subcortical pathways
for presentation.
The key point here in the Spanish sensory system, and
as we'll discover in a second in the visual system,
is that these signals are taken from the skin, in
this case through the spinal cord, taken up through the
thalamus, everything goes through the thalamus of cerebral cortex, and
they project upon what is called the primary somatic sensory
cortex.
It's not a sensory to touch primary because that's the
major source of input from the thalamus.
So the primary cortices are those part of the cerebral
cortex, the input from the thalamic relay cells for each
different modality.
And they form this, they project in this case onto
the present.
So we'll see in a second in the cerebral cortex.
By the way, a lot of what we know about
this is actually from work, from Walter Penfield and his
colleagues in McGill.
Back in the 1940s and fifties, where as they were
preparing patients for surgery for epilepsy, actually made in different
parts of the cerebral cortex.
And that's what the patients felt.
These experiments are no longer very often conducted, but they
were incredibly illuminating at the time.
They were able to map out in humans the structure
of this amount of sensory cortex, for example, by asking
people what they felt when you stimulated different parts of
the cortex.
So this is the central stool because these are going
to be the potential supertankers get some fees.
This is a central focus as part of the major
Suvi in the brain and before pre and post after
the simple superset is post mains behind towards the back
of the brain premiums in front was a frontal brain
and if you looked along this potential suitcase, you find
a structure which is incredibly beautiful.
What happens there is that if you stimulate different parts
of this personal sulcus, some person will report that they
feel sensations at different parts of their body.
So, for example, if you down on the lateral side
and you stimulate it, you might find that some reports
that they felt a sensation on their face.
Whereas if you're up on the top medial side, that
report, instead of sensation on that front or on the
foot or leg.
So depending on whereabouts alone, this person was suicidal whereabouts
and cerebral sucres are you are encoding.
You are representing activity on different parts of your skin.
I think this gives rise to the concept, which is
the homunculus in terms of the touch among us means
little man.
And if you look at the representation of the body
on this first episode, the touch representation, you find that
this continuous representation of different parts of the body and
different parts of the body that are closer together are
generally speaking, represented closer together on the cortex.
So if example, the foot is represented a similar location
to the leg or the trunk, whereas if face the
lips and the nose are represented close to each other
and in between these of that hand in the arm.
So you get this map of the body that's formed
on this matter sensory cortex.
It's the different types of touch that you can get
in different parts of your body as represented on the
similar sensory cortex.
And you find that actually some parts of the body
seem to be overrepresented.
For example, the face is a large part of the
sensory cortex, whereas the foot is quite a small part,
even though its size is relatively speaking, even not in
many cases in the face or the trunk, for example,
occupies almost a minuscule part of the cerebral cortex.
And the reason for that is fairly obvious if you
think of just one simple principle.
Every sensory receptor has about the same amount of cerebral
cortex devoted to it.
It follows then that we have more sensor receptors, for
example, on your finger, on your thumb, on your face,
you'll have more cerebral cortex devoted to that part of
the body.
And when you have less sensory receptors, it shrunk your
arm, your leg, you have less part of the body
devoted to that is what we would call cortical magnification.
The idea that the cerebral cortex is like a magnifying
glass onto your body.
That magnifying glass, how much it magnifies depend on the
density of the sensory receptors at that part of your
body.
This is what the locomotive looks like if you're trying
to represent it as an intact human being.
This cortical magnification has some implications.
It means that your ability, as I said, to detect
small changes in the position of objects or the presence
of two objects instead of one depends on the density
of sensory receptors and therefore the amount of cerebral cortex
that's actually devoted to that part of the skin.
This graph here shows you compares is perceptual, the psychophysical
acuity that you have the different parts of your body
and that low numbers in mean high acuity.
That means you're very sensitive to small distances and large
numbers being low acuity.
That means you in much larger distances between objects to
determine tunes at one there and can see that the
areas of the body with the largest or the lowest
acuity at a lower arm of the arm, shoulder, belly,
back breast, thigh, half all those areas where you know
yourself that when you touch those things, you're very less
tense, very much less sensitive to the structure of the
things that are touching that part of the body.
Whereas for example, the fingers and the upper lip, the
cheek, the nose have much higher acuity and much more
sensitive to different the structure of the things that are
touching the body.
So this density of receptors determines the amount of cerebral
cortex that is actually devoted to that part of the
sensory apparatus.
And that amount of cortex is devoted to that.
Sensory in turn dictates how sensitive or how how much
acuity you have, the sensory stimuli that impinge in that
part of the body.
I just briefly wanted to show you the structure, the
visual pathways, very similar.
I'm not going to go through all these lines.
I just want to illustrate to you from a paper
that we produced many years ago now, that in the
eye there are retinal nerve cells which include photoreceptors that
signal they're actually communicated by a little network of cells
in the retina, which you learn about later stages via
ganglion cells, whose axons make up the optic nerve.
Those ganglion cells in turn go to this little structure
in the thalamus the lateral clinic is, and from there
their signals projected the primary visual cortex or V1.
So very similar in structure to this, not a sensory
pathway, except that one went through the spinal cord to
get the thalamus and then the cortex or this one
from the eye or straight through the optic nerve, through
the thalamus and the visual cortex.
There are different pathways from the eye to the thalamus,
and that's to visual cortex.
We often call these the P of the M or
the PARTICELLE, and the Minnesota pathways is quite a bit
in the reading that I suggested you do that discusses
how the signals of these nerve cells differ.
I just want to introduce you to the idea that
they actually have different structure and different types of signals.
Now, I said on the cement essentially surface, you can
tell quite easily that the finger, for example, is what
you used to touch on things, and you can feel
the fine gradations in the texture, for example, the surface.
Similarly, if you look in the eye, if you take
a photo through the eye and this is what a
photo through the eye looks like, you see an object.
This is the bit where the optic nerve starts with
the axons of the ganglion cells come out of the
eye and go into the optic nerve.
It's also the place where the blood vessels come into
the eye from the optic nerve.
This is the picture of the eye through a fan
scope and in the middle of the eye.
See, the structure is called the phobia.
It's an incredible structure.
And this structure, you've got no blood vessels.
This is Photoshopped as a smaller than anywhere else in
the body.
And then the other apparatus in the retina has been
pushed away.
So the photos have to have direct access to the
light that comes through the lens and hits the retina.
And in this location, in this part of the eye,
the small part of the eye is about three or
four millimetres in size.
You have this incredibly dense population of nerve cells called
cone photoreceptors, and that's represented by this paper down here.
You can see the density of these cones peaks in
that area.
And that means that there's because there's so many different,
so many more photoreceptors in this particular part of the
eye that you can distinguish between an object that's slightly
displaced.
So when I wanted to see the structure of the
visual world, what I need to do is I need
to move my eyes around so that part of the
world that I'm interested in falls on the phobia.
Because then I can distinguish the difference between what might
be, for example, a happy face, a sad face, a
bald face.
So if I want to see that fine special detail,
someone at the back of the room who's sitting there
looking at me, that is about one quarter of my
thumb.
And is it slightly less than one degree of ice?
If I want to be able to distinguish the difference
between someone's eyes or their face to recognise their face,
I have to bring my phobia onto that object.
I have to move my eyes so that that part
of the visual falls onto my phobia where I have
this really dense array of photoreceptors.
I won't talk about them at all.
But the gods, the ones which are important for night
vision actually more dense just outside of the phobia.
And so it turns out if you're ever out there
in the dark night looking at the stars, if you
want to see a star, you don't look directly at
it, you slightly side of it.
And that's because of what photoreceptors are actually absent from
the photograph from the centre of your gaze.
And instead you need to bring that light onto the
side of your phobia with regard to actually most dense.
It turns out that this structure, this density photo receptors
in the in the phobia, which is so much greater
in the phobia than elsewhere in the eye, is paralleled
by changes in the structure of the subsequent nerve cells
in the eye.
And the consequence is that we can't see fine spatial
detail in the periphery of our vision.
This again is the idea of cortical magnification.
We can see fine spatial detail when we're looking directly
at something, but not when away from that centre of
our guys.
Our cortex has magnified that small part of the visual
field, which is occupied by the phobia.
I think that's about the size of your thumb.
That part of the visual field where all these thousands
of kind photoreceptors are sitting waiting for light to come.
The primary visual cortex is actually totally magnifying that part
of the visual field.
Some estimates would put it to be something like 20
or 30% of primary visual cortex is devoted to this
tiny little part of the visual field, and the rest
of the visual field is consequently represented by many fewer
ganglion cells, cells in the cortex.
So therefore we're much less capable of seeing the finer
spatial detail away from the centre of gaze because the
cortical magnification is so pronounced for us.
For real.
You can see this yourself.
If you have a look at this slight demonstration here.
If you look in the centre of this thing here
on the projector, you should be able to read.
If you look where the arrow is, you should be
able to define or distinguish what each of the different
letters is.
Does everyone agree with that?
Approximately.
If you look at the centre, you can still see
this on the left, the K on the right and
left from bottom on the top.
It's like going to.
I then if, on the other hand, you look at
the air over here, you should not be able to
define many of the letters.
They are present there.
So you may be able to see that the ace
in the S is there and you could see the
K in the R, But many of the things like
T.P. y are actually indecipherable to you.
And the reason for that is that what I've done
in making this diagram here is scale the size of
the letters so that they occupy approximately the same amount
of cerebral cortex when you're looking at the centre of
the diagram.
And because they'll find the same amount of cerebral cortex,
you're equally able to see all those letters.
But when we look at the eye and this is
no longer matched, sometimes that is a much smaller and
occupying enough of the cerebral cortex.
The magnification is wrong.
So this viewpoint.
So you are less able to be able to detect
what these different letters are.
Hmm.
I said that there were parallel pathways that take the
signals from the eye to the visual cortex, and these
are quite pronounced if you look in the thalamus.
You can see in this little structure overlap, which is
a beautiful structure.
I spent most of my life studying it.
So I think it's beautiful.
It's called that unique because it looks a bit like
a me.
If you're younger than me, actually bend your knee, then
you would look a little bit like me.
And in this little genic nucleus, which you can see
by the naked eye is these different layers or parts
of magnet.
So it layers smaller and larger cell bodies.
And it is these layers.
Now we know that these cells would communicate different things
to the cerebral cortex.
These different layers get different inputs from the eye, in
particular these ganglion cells, the cells that form apple to
the eye, to the thalamus.
The particular ones are much smaller.
They're going to pass through the magnet.
So everyone's a much larger.
They're going to be sterilised.
And then these signals of these thalamic neurones then go
to primary visual cortex.
Now, turns out there's very few ways to test this.
But the only in fact, the only way to really
test is supply small lesions, as we discussed last week.
You can place small lesions in the brain, in animals,
in a small controlled lesions, and you can destroy some
of the nerve cells in the palm.
So all the magnet, some of the layers.
And if you try an animal farm to report things
about the outside world and therefore ask whether or not
these nerve cells that come from the right things in
the retina and send them to the cerebral cortex with
these nerve cells saying different things, the cerebral cortex.
And it turns out they do.
So, for example, in this set of beautiful work is
Bill Murray and to conduct in the late 1980s and
90.
Summarised in this review that I cited here, the idea
is that there's a lesion this place in the palm
of the layers of the macaque monkey.
Or in the magnesium layers of the of a different
macaque monkey with monkey before the length made.
This monkey is trained to report simple things about the
outside world.
What the colour they trying to simply report.
What is something they will not.
In a particular location.
Hmm.
If there was something that I went to make one
important.
If there wasn't a to make another report.
These graphs here, the solid lines show the capacity of
the animal to detect something which varies either in the
kind of striping this of the patterns that are present
or in the flicker.
That is the kind of amount of times per second
something flickers, a light flickers.
Africa is defined by the tempo frequency.
That is how many times a second something, because the
spatial frequency here is just how many of these fine
bars you have in one degree of visual angle, the
solid limestone, what the monkey does in the normal case
without a lesion and the different points.
So the monkeys performance when you've after you've made a
lesion, it turns out that if you make a lesion
at the end pathway, you have almost no impact on
the monkey's capacity to detect the spatial form of an
object.
Whereas if you make a leap into the pathway, this
is almost most.
On the other hand, if you make a lesion at
the pathway you detect, you abolish the animal's capacities, the
black lines here to detect very rapidly flickering things.
Whereas if you abolish the pathway, we preserve the capacity
to detect those rapidly flickering things.
And finally, if you lesion the pathway, you kind of
the monkey cannot see coloured objects, whereas if you lesion
the pathway, the monkey can.
So these things, these different perceptual abilities in the presence
and absence of different pathways suggest that the different signals
that come from the eye to the cortex carry qualitatively
different and qualitatively different signals about the outside world that
carried along the parallel pathways to the cerebral cortex where
they are.
Then we combine.
So when we get to Cortex, we've got all these
parallel pathways doing stuff in the sensory periphery, whether in
visual cortex and other sensory cortex, wherever it is.
We have these parallel pathways from a sensory final thalamus,
bringing all this information up to the cortex.
And somehow the cortex has to rearrange these interesting judgements
about the outside world.
For many years.
Before that, the brain was basically you what you're born
with.
You had that there was no plasticity in the brain.
It took several decades of experiments to actually reject that
hypothesis.
I want to show you a couple of those experiments
in the substance lines on this line.
These are some of the very early experiments that were
able to reject the hypothesis that the brain, the cerebral
cortex in particular, was indifferent to experience.
It was the same when you were born as when
you were dying.
These two experiments relied again on work on monkeys because
these monkeys were easy enough to be trained to report
what had happened to to the outside world.
In the first experiment here, I want to say is
the monkey was trained to hold the fingers against a
little rotating disc, and I had to make judgements about
that rotating this, I think the direction of motion, that
rotating disk using the fingers only.
The question was, would the experience, the long term experience
of making this judgement with your fingers change the representation
of the fingers in the cerebral cortex?
And the way that the researchers went about trying to
address that question is that they made electrophysiological recordings from
this ninth century cortex of these monkeys before, during and
after training to do this simple task.
And what they find is summarised in this slide here,
which is a little bit complicated, but the end result
is very straightforward.
So this is recording from this mid-century area of an
al monkey, people that monkey, and they're recording from the
region that is most important in representing the hand associated
before it's a monkey.
Listen, the sensory cortex that's there now monkeys as it
is in humans.
What they did was they made recordings from these part
of the cortex before experience, and they found a particular
representation of the digits, the final four digits, five, which
of the hand in this part of the cortex.
And that's described over here.
So is the fifth, fourth, second or third digit.
And the normal here.
In the normal case, these different digits have approximately equal
parts of the cortex devoted to them.
However, following this experience, following this training, you find a
substantial overrepresentation of the second and the third digit, and
those are the two digit, the two fingers that the
animal is using to make this judgement.
So prolonged exposure to these kind of tasks has changed
how the cerebral cortex is organised.
It has increased the amount of cerebral cortex that's important,
that's used for extending information from those two digits.
The animals used to do the task.
So the brain is plastic.
The organisation, the cerebral cortex is plastic.
It can adapt to the structure of experience and tasks
that we would need to accomplish.
Although a substantial amount of work as shown this many
different systems since, and this was the original work to
show that this plasticity was there.
I really encourage you to read it with a beautiful
set of experiments.
The converse is it can also be studied.
That is what happens when you lose a digit or
lose some part of your sensory periphery.
In this case, if the animal, for example, loses a
third digit.
This case is surgically removed and the seizure.
Again, you can make the recordings before and after that
surgery.
And again, you can measure in this case again from
one case in the parts of the brain that represent
the hand.
And you find, at least in some cases and this
is still controversial, that when the third digit is removed,
a bit of the cerebral cortex that was responsible for
including things that happened on that anger now include things
that happened on the second or the fourth or the
ninth and fingers.
It's like this piece of cerebral cortex wants to do
something, wants to do anything.
And in the absence of any input from the third
digit, it's asking for input from the second and the
fourth digits help the brain represent things that are going
on there.
So the brain is the cerebral cortex is plastic.
It can adapt to changes in the input from the
outside world.
Say this is not uncontroversial.
In some systems, this seems to be less the case,
and some systems seem to be more the case.
It is certainly dependent on what kind of life injury
happened early.
Younger people who suffer injuries have more cortical plasticity.
Older people have less.
One of the things that this leads to is phantom
limb.
And I just want to spend a couple of seconds
showing you a really effective video from one of the
leaders in this field.
Ramachandran.
I find this quite an effective video, so just spend
a couple minutes on.
Twins First patients this Derek Steen.
All right.
One of Ramachandran first patients was Derek Steen.
13 years ago, he was involved in a motorcycle accident,
and I pulled the nerves out of my spinal cord
up in my neck.
They told my parents directly that I would never use
my arm again.
About seven years ago, I was reading through the classifieds
and I saw an ad in there.
Amputees wanted that.
It was a joke like that.
It's just basically connecting the club to the ball.
So I called the number and it was Dr. Ramachandran.
Go relax.
Today, Derek is teaching Ramachandran how to play golf.
But several years ago, Derek made a crucial contribution to
Ramachandran pioneering work in brain science.
Yes, I was amazing.
After my surgery, I sat up in the bed and
still felt the arm there.
Still felt everything there.
And I'm looking down and I'm seeing nothing.
It was pretty bizarre.
The more I thought about it, the more it hurt.
The more it hurt, the more I thought about it.
So it was it was like it was never ending.
I mean, I'd break out in a cold sweat and
turn pale.
Just standing here talking to you because the pain would
hit so bad.
If there is any one thing about our existence that
we take for granted.
It's the fact that we have a body.
Each of us has a body.
And, you know, you give it a name, it has
a bank account and so on and so forth.
But it turns out even your body is something that
you construct in your mind.
And this is what we call your body image.
Now, of course, in my case, it's substantiated by the
fact that I really use a body with bone and
tissue.
But the sense I have, the internal sense I have
of the presence of a body and arms and all
of that is, of course, constructed in my brain and
it's in my mind.
And the most striking evidence for this comes from these
patients who have had an amputation and continue to feel
the presence of the missing.
How?
It was the beginning of an important relationship.
Important for Derek, because not only would he finally understand
his phantom pain, he would also get to the bottom
of a mysterious sensation he felt while shaving.
When I first started shaving after my surgery, I would
feel my absent hand start to hurt and tingle whenever
I shaved this left side of my face.
Meeting Derrick was important for Ramachandran because the explanation he
came up with would rock the world of neuroscience.
Photograph.
That's just my arm.
The first thing Ramachandran did was to invite Derek to
his lab for a simple test that I want to
touch different parts of your body.
And I just want you to tell me what you
feel and where you experience the sensation.
Close your eyes.
I could feel that on my forehead.
Anything anywhere else?
No.
So my nose.
Okay.
My chest.
Your chest.
Okay.
I can feel that on my cheek and I can
feel rubbing on the phantom left hand.
On the phantom left hand in addition to your cheek,
I'm going to run the Q-Tip across your jaw and
see what happens.
I can feel like you did by my cheek and
I can feel a stroking sensation across the phantom hand.
You actually feel that stroking across your phantom hand.
Okay, so that small visual video and you can just
it goes on for a while.
I encourage you to watch it.
That shows the fact that this person has lost their
arm, that some part of their representation of their body
has distorted not just the inside of the brain, but
also perceptually cognitively.
And the likely explanation for this is that one part
of the likely explanation for this is that the representation
of the face is actually quite close to the representation
of the hand.
And as we saw with the monkey who is missing
the third digit, when you lose inputs to certain parts
of the cerebral cortex, that cortex seems to want to
do something anyway, stop to draw input from neighbouring cortical
areas.
So that part of the body which was representing the
arm is now no longer there.
Now he's also drawing input from the face as clearly
more complicated than just simply saying that because this person's
body image is constructed, image is something that is not
simply explained just by the amount of sensory cortex, but
that distortion in the cortical representation is going to contribute
to the fact that this person feels something, even though
there is no longer there is plasticity is important in
helping this, in helping the brain effectively try to reconstruct
or to do what it would like to do, even
in the absence of inputs.
I just want to spend a couple of minutes facing
what will be spending most of the next lecture on
or the next one.
And I'd really like you to do some reading in
the next section, which is this review that I've put
up online from Colby and go back to leaders in
the field reviews to look at old.
Now that is probably the best conceptualisation of the ideas
we'll go through in the next lecture.
We discussed that, that sometimes that is kind of a
frame of reference in which you understand these sensations is
is depends on how you want to think about things.
I just want to explain to you what I mean
by frames of reference for a few slides.
So when we look at the cerebral cortex, we see
this translated into many distinct areas and visual cortex, for
example, as primary visual cortex.
But then there's about three or four, maybe even ten
or 15 different visual areas that sit next to primary
visual cortex, the whole higher order cortical areas or association
cortex.
The same is the case in similar sensory cortex, the
same effects in the auditory cortex.
You have these primary areas, then you have these multiple
other satellite areas.
And the question arises, one that actually puzzled researchers for
many decades now is why do you have so many
cortical areas?
Why don't we just have one area that's responsible for
vision, one area that's responsible for autism?
And the hypothesis that I'd like to explore in the
next lecture is very much like these parallel pathways from
the sensory periphery to cerebral cortex.
These different cortical areas act as parallel representations or parallel
constructions of the outside world.
Each area is doing something, creating a slightly different interpretation
of the outside world.
This then raises the question of how can these different
the things that are arising in these different cortical areas
be brought back together?
How can the different maps of the outside, both the
construction of the outside world, be reconciled?
And the second thing that starts when I ask is
these topographical photographs and statements, these things that are maps
of your body, your maps of your eyes, that's fine
If we want to, you know, represent the precise location
in our body that something happens.
But it's not very useful if we want to move
around the world where I need to know where my
location is with respect to this table.
With respect to the microphone.
With respect to these chairs.
So the question that arises is how these topographic maps
of the sensory body of the body of the eye,
how these transformed into something that could be behaviourally useful,
could actually help us move around the world accomplish tasks.
It was not very useful.
Just simply know that this is a place of my
hand or my arm.
Sorry.
I would like to know where that place is with
respect.
For example, if my arm is moved with respect to
the rest of my body.
So I want to order frame a frame of reference
in which I can understand these different aspects of my
movement throughout the world.
And that's that is the majority past of what we
call the parietal cortex.
And that's what we're going to be spending the next
half, the next lecture on.
And as I said, I'd really like you to read
that coding review because that will help you understand what
it is that the cortex is trying to do, how
it's constructing maps of the outside world that we can
use to move around them.
And so we, we, we investigated that on Friday, and
I look forward to seeing you there.
Thanks.
Yeah.
I was.
Yes.
So it's and particularly in two volumes.
It was.
Particularly a good topic for the show.
But something about.
Are you actually doing.
It where it's kind of.
Surprising for you to feel particular?
I can't quite remember where where we are at with.
I think the general sensation of like, I'm going to
take on you, that's relatively easy enough to understand.
This is the migration and it's kind of been associated
with the reaction.
You know how it is that.
According to what's called a predictive coding framework in the
world.
Where you can predict quite well what the temptation is
that you.
Should get.
Right because you're doing.
Something and you can because you know what you should
be getting ready to.
Predict and therefore surprise what is predictable.
And think there's a framework of understanding brain function, which
Professor SEO has been particularly.
Important from guidance.
Which is that the job of the brain, you basically
find out things that are not.
And so a lot of the architecture, the brains that
Christians are predicting, that includes, you know, perception, suppressing, not
encoding things that, you know.
I mean, for example, I think that.
You can suppress your own sensations, especially during actions.
So that's predictive protein, which is really influential in.
On the questions regarding the object.
Yes.
So yes, in exactly the same way.
It's easier to see the things overlap altogether.
Like, yeah.
That's where you can.
Something.
I think, optimism.
And because here.
And in my.
You.
Yeah.
Yeah.
The overlapping.
For one thing, the idea of hiring people sometimes out.
I'm very.
Part of life, as you can imagine, is just a
single.
You would.
But even in this case.
Consequently.
That often.
That changes.
Central brain function.
Yes.
Like many.
And so even when you figure.
It was final.
Expect to really point and explain everything.
Thank you.
Thank you.
So.
And would the to.
Yeah, I look at that because it's so much easier
to like.
Yeah.
Okay.
Yeah.
You know, like in our.
I mean, the idea is to.
Always have an interactive story, but I don't know the
exact exact.
I like.
I felt.
I don't know.
I mean.
Yes.
Okay.
And I knew it would be like.
Well, this.
So people.
And.
We know.
Know.
I.
You know this.
Yeah.
Yeah.
Oh.
Yeah.
People.
I think.
And.