y2clutch / raw_transcripts /lecture_5.txt
elyx's picture
initial commit
dae990d
raw
history blame
46 kB
So that's best thing.
Dr. Richard Francis.
That's the they to take the word.
What is.
Hello.
Welcome back.
If you are.
My name's Sam Solomon.
It's my pleasure also to introduce to you today Professor
Elliott Carton.
He'll be taking a lot of this lecture.
So in the last two weeks, we've discussed the different
components of brains and brain function.
The purpose of this week in general is to help
you understand a little bit better to survey the variety
of techniques that are available now to study brains and
behaviour.
To set us up properly for the subsequent weeks, which
were on specific terms.
So the purpose of today is not to go into
in-depth any particular technique.
We just like to help you understand the panoply of
techniques that are available with the strengths and the limitations
of some of those techniques are and why one might
choose to employ them in particular circumstances to understand the
relationship between brains and behaviour.
This is now quite an old slide, or at least
most of it is.
I adapted it slightly on the x axis is the
timescale of which one might like to make a measurement
ranging from milliseconds through hours, days and even lifetimes.
On the y axis.
It's a spatial scale of which one might like to
make that measurement from the scale of a single snaps
of a single nerve.
So through two o columns of those whole brain areas
and perhaps even the whole brain, each of the little
things in the box are different types of techniques, most
of which will encounter some time today.
These include things that are, for example, called patch clamp
electrophysiology.
This is old school electrophysiology, incredibly powerful technique.
You put in a glass electrode, so its tip is
only 1000 or maybe 5000 millimetre wide.
You put that glass tip up against the membrane of
a single nerve cell in a slice usually, but sometimes
in a whole animal.
You suck that little membrane onto the end of the
pipette and then you break the seal from the glass
into the inside of the cell, what's called a giga
arm seal.
And that allows you to measure precisely the internal electrical
life of that.
So that's basically the smaller scale measurement techniques tend to
get through on the top.
Right, as we'll be discussing soon, is a kind of
knowledge you can gain from studying humans or animals with
lesions, maybe to large parts of the brain or to
even small parts of the brain, often formed by strokes
or other kinds of accidents.
And in between.
These are a range of different techniques that we'll be
encountering today, including electrophysiology, calcium imaging, optical imaging that Magneto
and Safflower Graham or the electroencephalogram MEG or EEG, but
also techniques that are not necessarily aimed at revealing the
functional activity of the brain, but the kind of connections
between different brain areas.
If we were to have this lecture back when the
slide was first made.
I think the slide was first made back in 1994
because there had been some profusion of techniques, including MRI,
into the field.
These techniques have now even grown more stronger and more
varied in their ability to answer these kinds of questions.
Hopefully we'll give you a flavour of that today, so
I'll hand off now to the radio to take you
through the first component, which is largely to do with
how we try and measure brain function in human beings.
Hello, everybody.
Can you hear me?
Yeah.
Hi.
It's really nice to be here.
I'm Andrea.
I'm one.
This is a professor of experimental psychology, and I mostly
do research on human cells quite nicely to telling you
about some of the techniques that we use.
And we wanted to start a little bit with.
Well, the first thing that was available for us to
study the human brain.
Right?
And that was sort of lesions in patients.
And that's something that's very much like we call neuropsychology,
although neuropsychology implies all those things as well.
And you're going to be seeing this a lot through
your modules.
And so I'm not going to go into a lot
of detail about it, but it is fair to say
that sort of the groundbreaking studies of Paul Broca were
the ones that sort of get this field going in
many ways.
So Rocco had a patient that had some language production
issues, and when that patient, that patient died and then
they examined the brain, they saw that they had a
lesion in the left hemisphere, in the frontal charges, in
the frontal cortex.
And they could also see that that happened again with
a couple of patients.
So that was the first real sort of link between
a function behaviour and sort of something localised to a
certain part of the brain.
And that was really groundbreaking for for neuroscience and for
psychology.
And it is the technique that we still use up
to today.
We're going to we're going to talk a lot about
hair and burn care during the language recognition lectures.
And we also are going to talk about a fascia
and how we still use neuropsychology to study the brain
in humans.
So as I said, it's just really good in terms
of getting this still going and making sort of relationships
between structure and function in the brain.
And what we can do now is also say, okay,
well, we have these Asian to have this lesion.
Let's try to see how they perform different tasks.
And again, you begin to see this a lot.
A classic example, the studies of our friend Amelia with
patients, H.M. and.
So yeah, really groundbreaking as well.
That went to a lot of studies.
We learned a lot about memory from those and that
this technique obviously, you know, has advantages which are like
and how we can link brain, um, behaviour and potentially
identify regions that are necessary and lesions that never sort
of sort of constrained to certain regions of the brain
if you want.
We don't really know what's damage.
The damage will be different between patients.
So it's a little bit, it's a kind of like
what people call sometimes a nature experiment.
Well, someone had a listen.
Let's try to understand what happened.
And I think in terms of disadvantages was that it
was that the lesions are not selective and that also,
obviously, the brain has this this capacity of plasticity of
of change in potentially structure and function to compensate for
what's happening environments is what happened in its own physiology.
And so there are a lot of cases that actually
the brain compensates over time.
So we see this a lot in patients and you
will say the cases of aphasia were potentially at the
beginning after lesion.
A patient has like several sort of potential issues with
producing or understanding language.
Let's say that as time passes, many of them recover.
So not all of them.
And that's like a real interesting or like a big
area of research trying to understand why a patient will
not recover.
Many of them do.
And that has to do with how the brain can
compensate for that damage.
However, there are lots of things that you can't really
do.
You can't really study dynamics between brain regions because you
know that lesion is already damaged.
So, you know, that's the only thing that you know.
And and I think I think for me, the biggest
one is that, well, deletion is not selective.
So you wouldn't know what exactly is damage or to
what extent.
And therefore, it's very difficult to make sort of inferences
between the structure and the function.
We can do it by studying sort of large groups
of patients.
But for example, there is currently study in Queens at
UCL where they're trying to look at that Netflix of
yeah, so people would have patients are going to be
all the brain lesions and therefore sort of language production
and perception problems and they're trying to look at what
predicts recovery and they really need to recruit thousands of
patients to be able to do this properly.
So it is, it is quite hard.
Okay.
So I'm going to talk now about some other techniques
that we can use to actually measure brain function and
structure in healthy individuals and in a non-invasive way.
And the first one I'd like to talk to you
about is magnetic resonance imaging.
And I'm just going to show you a video that
explains this very well from one of the developers of
this technique.
If you take, let's say, a human being and put
him in one of these big, very homogeneous magnetic field
magnets, then there's a tendency of that magnetic field to
line up the magnetic moments of the nuclei, the spin
of the nuclei and the hydrogen in your body, which
is in your muscle and in your blood.
Now, put on a radio frequency pulse, say 60 megahertz
or something like that.
Then you can make this magnetisation of your hydrogen nuclei.
You can turn it 90 degrees away from the direction
of the magnetic field.
Your magnetic moment will process.
If you have coils around, pick up coils, they it
will induce a signal.
If I want to see where the signal is coming
from in your body, I put on another magnetic field
on top of the very homogeneous one that's called a
magnetic field gradient.
By that I mean it makes a feel stronger in
one place and weaker in another place.
What you do in order to actually get the image
that you want, the fan resolution that you need, that
you put on magnetic field gradients of different strengths, a
series of pulses.
That's why if you get an MRI machine, you're here.
Bom, bom, bom.
It's all these pulses that we're putting out with different
strengths of magnetic field, perhaps even different directions, because we
want to get a three dimensional picture of you.
You record all of these data and when you finish,
you can now use what's called Fourier transform.
It's a mathematical technique.
You can work back to how strong the signal was
in each of these voxels.
And so this is how the image is developed.
Okay.
So that was a very quick intro to.
Right.
So am I in the wrong one?
Okay, so MRI stands for magnetic resonance imaging, and we
usually use this really big magnets, scanners to to sort
of scan people to conduct this technique.
And functional MRI refers to the kind of specific scanning
that is used for measuring, you know, something that relates
to brain function.
And I what I'm going to expand as I'm going
to explain both of them.
And so how I.
It's just plain.
Old.
Okay.
Thank you for your input on my music selection.
And so in Ryan ephemera and material I'm going to
talk about a little bit later, it really changed the
game in terms of what we could do to study
the human brain.
And in the last 25 years that these techniques have
been around, we have gained a lot of understanding of
how many functions that we can only study in detail
in humans, such as potential language or some complex decision
making or, you know, metacognition and so on, how they
work and how how, how the brain represents or like
produces its functions.
And so how does an MRI what I mean to
me, it's absolutely amazing that we can look into the
people's brain and actually look into their function without even
putting anything inside.
We can just take pictures of their brains by measuring
how hydrogen atoms move, which is why I was trying
to say here.
So this is what happens.
You put the subject a the participant in a big
magnets.
The magnets and had a really, really strong magnetic field.
And to do that you need liquid helium and you
need to keep that constant all the time.
So the energy cost of doing this is quite high.
And that's why MRI is actually a really expensive technique.
And when you put a person in this big magnetic
field, as it was thought by all the sort of
elite hydrogen atoms were aligned in the same direction.
Right.
That's why you don't use trying to put them all
in the same direction.
So then when you sort of disturb them and when
a radio wave.
Right, they will sort of rotate.
When you turn that off, they're going to go back.
And when they go back, they're going to miss the
signal that you can think about sort of like a
kind of singing in a certain frequency.
You can see these electrons.
And so these hydrogen atoms are thinking in a special
frequency when they're going back to the alignment.
Right.
And that's why we're measuring now how how then can
we measure different parts of space if they're all aligned
in the same way?
That's where you introduce these gradients.
So the gradient is just changing the magnetic field slightly.
So in a way, you can think about these hydrogen
atoms sort of kind of moving and what this thing
that's going to be sent is going to be at
a different frequency.
You can think about it as thinking in a different
frequency.
So for example, if I wanted to know, you know,
how this lecture theatre was populated, but I couldn't see
it, I was in a different room.
I could just put microphones all around with different with
different frequencies and then just have a recording here.
Right.
So the guys at the very top are going to
have really low frequencies, and that's going to be a
great note here to very high frequencies.
So I'm going to go in the other room and
she's going to be recording the signals from all those
microphones when you're talking or whatever.
And by looking at the frequencies of those recordings, I'm
going to be able to exactly say who was sitting
where.
Because that's the link between frequency and space.
And that's why MRI dogs and that's why I like
to see pictures of your brain, which is amazing.
So if there's like a really strong signal at a
specific frequency, I know where it's coming from.
That part of of their image is going to look
quite bright.
And if it's a really low frequency, then it's going
to look like that.
And that's how we construct and it's really nice brain
pictures.
And so, yeah, pictures like this, for example.
Right.
So you can see and this is just a transformation
of light, really strong powers.
These things are like y by looking wide or depending
on what y your technique is, depending on the contrast.
And this the opposite for things that, you know, they
don't have that much signal.
Now this is just to get a structural image.
This just gives you a picture of the brain, right,
That doesn't link at all to brain function.
So what is it that allowed us to use functional
MRI to look at brain function?
And it is the fact that when a certain part
of the brain is active, there will be an increase
in blood flow to that part of the brain.
When there is an increase in blood flow, there's going
to be more haemoglobin, haemoglobin coming into that and that
has iron.
And it's in a sense the iron is ferromagnetic so
interferes with that magnetic field.
So by measuring how much interference there is with the
magnetic field, we can measure brain function.
But as you can see, the images and I'm sure
you hear one is the structural image to the kind
of images that we use for studying the structure of
the brain.
And this is a functional image.
It's just very low resolution because we actually want to
see.
How it changes with time.
So acquiring one of these images it takes for the
whole brain, it takes at least 5 minutes, whereas acquiring
this one could take 2 to 3 seconds depending on
what you're doing.
So we kind of compromising some of the spatial definition
to have like a better temporal resolution.
But we're still measuring blood flow, and blood flow is
quite slow.
That's why ephemeral is not a good technique for measuring
brain function.
So and you think it's no good technique for measuring
sort of Air Force temporal events is not it doesn't
have a great temporal resolution.
That being said, last week there was this really exciting
paper published where they had an MRI technique and so
milliseconds resolution now like very fast.
And it was done in animal models and it was
thought like in a single slice.
So it's not the whole brain, it's just the very
first step towards this technique.
But I think, you know, everybody in the field is
really exciting because it really looks like in a few
years, like maybe ten, 20, who knows?
We are going to have a technique that allows like
a really great temporal resolution potentially to study the human
brain as well.
And so when you get these images where you do
in the functional case and ephemera is that you take
many, many, many of them and then you compare them
across conditions.
So, for example, let's say here I will record like
lots of images do one of my goodness conditions of
my experiment, which could be it could be a memory
experiment, right?
So it could be like memorise these items.
And then I have a control condition where I just
tell people to, you know, look at them, don't memorise
them.
And then you can look at how like activity changes
across the whole brain.
Or like in here you can like look specifically into
a certain region, right?
So this region here is a few voxels, a voxel
instead of the minimal units and ephemera you can look
at as a three dimensional pixel.
And then we can average, for example, the activity through
that.
So if you look at this, the intensity in these
conditions is going to be different.
But this is something that you can't really tell, you
know, just by looking at.
And the difference is very, very small.
It's usually around, you know, between 1% to like 4%
or 10%.
Best case.
And and yeah, it's it's really small, but it is
very consistent.
So you can measure that and you can measure how
it changes.
And that's what we're doing here.
We have like these three measurements here, for example, from
that region in red, and then we compare them to
the measurements during the second condition and averages and see
this a difference in the amount of like bull's signal
in that area.
And by doing that across the whole grain, we can
come up with these statistical maps where we're showing where
in the brain there are significant differences between conditions.
Okay.
And so as I said, FEMA has brought lots of
advantages.
I think one of the really nice things is that
we can study the dynamics of the whole brain.
And, you know, that doesn't only apply to humans, but
it's also been used in animal models for this specific
purpose as well.
And after certain spends, we can understand how different parts
of the brain interact and how they form functional networks.
So we can like sort of do more complex analysis
of network dynamics and see how reaches interact under different
situations.
And you could do things that we couldn't do for
like with patients because, you know, patients essentially have some
sort of behavioural deficits.
So like there's things that they wouldn't be able to
do that we could do now because they're healthy participants.
And I'm very nicely we can think we can study
human brains, right?
We can study things that are so specific to humans
that would be difficult to study in all that and
which models.
So, for example, I study deafness and how the brain
of deaf individuals changes.
And you know, when the congenitally deaf and continues with
deafness in many cases results in a delay in language
acquisition, which then has a lot of impact on cognitive
functions as well.
So it's not that animals are not great for answering
those types of questions, so great for studying the effects
of sensory deprivation or lack of sensory experience, in this
case, a hearing and in the development of the brain
in plasticity.
And so it not so much to study what are
the impacts on cognitive processes.
And I think the best example of how we gain
from ephemera is we can do things like communicating with
patients in a coma.
And I really recommend you to look at these videos
from Agent Owen and his group where they are using
ephemera and what we know about from right.
They have been able to communicate with and with patients
that before we didn't have any insight into their entire
health status.
And so that has been a really amazing ground breaking
discovery.
So I'm not going to go into detail here right
now of time.
And there are tons of limitations, though.
And again, I think know, we have some understanding of
what the f MRI signal is.
So is this mix of like presynaptic activity and like
sort of cells firing as well.
And like, we don't know which ones it's coming from.
So you never really know what type of activity you
measure.
And you've just seen a difference in overall blood flow
in one region to the other.
So that is quite limiting.
And and again, the spatial resolution is great, you know,
like in comparison with other things.
But still, like we're looking at a bunch of cells
like and it's going to be quite hard to tell
what individual cells or small networks of self are actually
doing.
And and again, it's like this really big magnets.
So people have to stay, you know, sort of still
they can move around and that just gives limitations as
well.
I think, as some said, there's been a lot of
advances in these techniques and now we have MRI for
humans where we can measure layer specific activity.
So different layers of the and, you know, the cerebral
cortex.
But still, you know, that's still a lot of cells
and not specific.
Now, before we pass into other techniques, I wanted to
talk a little bit about this technique called functional, and
yet infrared spectroscopy, which works a little bit like ephemera
in sense that it measures changes in blood oxygenation.
So you have different sources of light that are attached
to the skull, and then you have others that you
use for detecting them.
And again, if there's a change in blood flow because
of haemoglobin, that will that will affect how the light
is reflected.
So you can measure changes as well.
Now, the spatial resolution of this technique is way worse
than if it were high, right?
Because you just only have a few seconds.
So why am I talking about it?
What would be the advantage of having this technique?
Why do you think?
Any ideas?
Yeah.
Yeah.
So exciting.
So one of the advantages that you don't need is
a big piece of equipment and is way more mobile.
Yeah.
People.
But you were always trying to.
Are you?
So hospital?
Yeah, exactly.
They don't have to be like in a specific lab
as well, although many cases are.
That is exactly that is one of the advantages.
So you're sacrificing some spatial resolution to actually be able
to do things like, you know, study people moving around
or like study children that are not going to be
staying very still in an MRI scanner.
And I think these are the kind of decisions that
we as scientists need to make, as we want to
learn about something, we're going to discover something about specific
behaviour, about the brain.
What's what is the best approach for doing this, given
the options that I have?
So in cases where you want to study brain function
in children, for example, or for example here where you
want to sort of like study people interacting and moving
around the environment, then obviously it's is going to be
a better technique that if I'm right.
Um, okay, so there are other techniques that are kind
of the opposite of around some that they have great
temporal resolution but not so good spatial resolution.
And those are magnetic photography and electroencephalography and energy and
energy.
So EEG have been around for a, for ages and
that was kind of the main technique that was used
to study the human brain in sort of healthy individuals
before any CnF MRI arrive other than just behavioural studies,
of course.
And so how do they what that means?
Just yeah, Okay.
So if you have a group of neurones, let's say
a line like it will be the case in the
cortex, they will have like went in different conditions but
actually they will have these currents moving in one direction.
Right.
And that is what generates EEG signal.
That's like the actual electrical activity.
And at the same time there will be a magnetic
field form surround that current that has gone in one
direction.
And that's why you can detect with energy.
And I think where you can see the main difference
in this really retro slide that I call it, because
it just happens quite clearly is that if you have
this current here, like in the case of ETI, it
doesn't go like and in sort of predictable way into
the skull because there is like, you know, sort of
bones and other tissues and liquid and so on, it
kind of deteriorates in a way that is quite difficult
to understand where it's coming from.
So you can still measure it here, but it's hard
to say where it's coming from.
And that's why EEG has a pretty bad spatial resolution
for energy.
This is much better because it is somehow more predictable
but still difficult to understand where exactly in the brain
this these signals are generated from.
And there's been a lot of work in trying to
do that.
And there have been some advantages based like kind of
I mean, when you talk to people that try to
understand this and try to sort of generate models of
where signals are coming from an EEG, and they basically
say this is an impossible problem to solve.
Like, you know, we can have like a really good
like closed solution as possible, but there's always going to
be a few different options.
So you're going to have to make decisions in terms
of how that now vanishes.
There's that that because I mentioned electrical activity, basically, you
know, this is direct recording of electrical activity, of neurones
firing or the magnetic field is generated and the temporal
resolution is excellent.
So again, you might want to try to understand what
experiments you want to do and whether, you know, this
technique is the best.
One of these techniques is by a technique that MRI,
for example.
And so I think I just want to show you
again, you can see there's a real difference in set
up here as well.
So energy, again, has like this really big machine, huge
magnetic field.
There has to be sort of maintain you could helium.
So again, quite expensive, but it now has to stay
really, really still whereas EEG is quite portable.
You know you can have a cap it's way cheaper
as well.
So trust me, you took my meds, you were like
the v0y is the best thing that I could do.
And then you have limitations of what your budget is
as well.
So, you know, that comes into it as well.
And so sometimes you might want to use one or
the other.
And I kind of did this sort of comparison here
to show you what are some of those things that
you will have to take into account.
So they both have excellent temporary solution, but similarly, they
both have problematic spatial resolution, although as I say, energy
is way better, right?
However, it is way more expensive and participants have to
stay very, very still.
I would say that applies to each year to some
extent as well, but potentially less and the sense.
US.
I'm in this big machine and that sort of how
the people haven't thought.
So you can't really move around.
So you need this special lab as well.
Whereas EEG is way more but more mobile and that
spatial resolution is was.
And I think it's also is good to take into
account that the signals come from different places as well.
So I'm not going to go into that.
But there are differences in where they're generated as well.
And and I think what is really nice as well
is that right now here at UCL in Queen Square,
in collaboration with all the labs in the UK, there
are developments to do with some portable e.g. such as
have like a few sensors that potentially measure brain activity
in a specific part of the brain so that people
can be more mobile and potentially have this much better
temporal resolution, so much better spatial resolution with this really
good temporal resolution as well.
And so I think this is the main so summary
of things that you can use for measuring brain activity
in humans.
I would really recommend you to watch all of these
videos, professors from our department talk more about these techniques.
And I think more once there's this is not an
extensive coverage of all the techniques that you can use
to study brain behaviour is more just to give you
a flavour of why the possibilities are and what are
some of the considerations that you have to take into
account when choosing one of those techniques?
So I'm going to pass to some now.
So it's going to be nice.
So the structure of this session is a little bit
maybe obscure for you.
Just to make clear why Bailey is talking about some
things that I'm talking about.
Other things is a video works on humans, particularly if
humans.
But I was always it's not quite always exclusively.
I grew up as a scientist working on primates, non-human
primates.
Both macaque monkeys and marmoset monkeys.
And I then moved when I moved to UCLA.
One of the reasons I moved here is that you
is basically the world centre of focus.
But for trying to understand the massive small world they
introduced in the last week.
Writing about half of France €70 million.
So the question I would like to pursue is what
I want you to think about.
Why would I choose to study animals?
And if I'm studying animals, including mine, which seems to
be so different from.
Why that I'm in the experimental psychology department.
Not only can I keep ahead of the problem.
So I'd like to then explain to you why I
find this such a beautiful experimental technique.
It's really exploded over the last ten years or so.
I'm going to introduce you to some of those techniques
that are really appropriate for the last ten years, and
we will be going into those in more detail.
We've actually.
But basically the reason we use animals or the reason
we turned out in some kind to understand why we
make based from them.
By that I mean that we can and.
Make small holes in their brain and introduce devices that
we can then use to measure cellular function in those
apple.
We can never be able to cover.
Study.
Study.
So.
But why do my people wear what I love about
Merhi?
Even in the best case scenario, one includes something like
$100,000.
But we can look at the active.
You need to be in the range and find out
how that could be.
That single macro might relate to cognitive functions.
If the animals, the kinds of things that do have
probably going to be very.
For example, we're in the very early parts of the
program.
People that critics are similarly enamoured with.
That makes me wonder.
I mean.
And I think we can learn a lot about how
we see or hear, or at least the signals that
come from that is or is at the centre of
the brain.
It's less clear how much we can learn about cognitive
structure thought, for example, by studying animals.
So it is when meant when we're measuring in invasively
would not be very limited circumstances that if we have
time we'll get back to at the end of this
lecture able to make these recordings from humans and he's
already introduced electroencephalogram or EEG.
That's a scalp based measurement, something that's non-invasive.
All the other things here, invasive measures, we might measure
the electrocardiogram, this liquid surface dura between the scalp and
the brain measuring almost directly neurones line underneath the electrodes.
Or we could use a little invasive, like a little
piece of silicon.
Let it be inserted into the brain first.
Slowly.
And located a particular point in the brain.
These have different spatial scales of measurement.
Electrocardiogram from each of the electrodes from the surface of
the brain, intermediate between B, g, and something else.
So maybe it measures that mm accumulated the brain issue.
The local feel and the electric car they find at
the end of one of the electrons moving about the
activity of about maybe half of their brain tissue.
And then if your electrodes, these little wires on fit
have been made well enough, you can actually start to
see the.
Speaking to people there for 2022 that you that in
the next slide we call those things spikes.
So this might be the signal that you would you
would acquire from one of the white actor to.
And the top.
This is 3 to 3 traces of the same trace.
On the top is what we would estimate as the
local fuel potential.
That's the sum that at the end of the start
to activity between the records and the ground, my whole
area.
And you can see that normally it's a local film
that was varying fairly slowly as the red line traces
out some of the slow fluctuations.
It's traced maybe a second or 3 seconds long.
And you can see that it is dominated by these
two things that are going.
But it would be very gratifying to deal with these
fluctuations if we then are able to look at those
little rapid events and those are indicated by the red
dots here.
We will see that they are the extracellular signal of
the action that we discussed in the last lecture.
How next?
Looks like from outside the city mirror.
There's not quite.
Now because his electrode is still quite large and there's
many cells around the end of those action potentials will
come from one another.
It doesn't really look.
Really?
Okay.
This better here.
Okay, I'll do that.
Thank you.
So the point to hear the difference down here, sir.
Thank you for that.
Each of each of those made.
It is like five or ten nerve cells around the
tip of the electrode.
Each of them are producing action potentials occasionally.
Often you cannot distinguish between the shapes of the action
potentials.
Just find your own one on your own.
Two on your own.
Three on your own.
For.
So we can combine these action potentials and say, well,
they're coming from single nerve cells.
It's just a few of them.
Five or ten of those may be in the local
area.
And therefore, we would call this activity the multi-unit activity.
So there's multiple units.
And by the way, you will come across this word
unit quite frequently in this course.
That just means a nerve cell in the brain recorded
in this way.
But sometimes if the electorate really is really nice and
it's really close to the so some of her neurones
in the brain tissue, you see a reproducible waveform.
Looks the same every time you identify in the recording
and that we would call single unit activity the activity
of a single nerve cell, the same nerve cell on
each turn, so able to record with this piece of
wire or multiple pieces of wire.
The activity of these nerve cells in the whole living
brain, in animals that are awake, behaving, even moving around
all environments.
So we can.
I think that this technique, which has been around now
for several decades, try to recall the activity of nerve
cells in the brain.
Oops.
This video shows you a video from the laboratory of
Torsten Wiesel and David Hubel trained maybe 50 years ago.
They won the Nobel Prize for these recordings.
These videos are hard to find, so I'd like to
show it here.
You're going to hear you can actually play those phrases
I showed you through an audio speaker and you can
actually listen to the action potentials that are being fired
by neurone.
In this case, they're recording from the visual cortex of
a cat.
And they're able to work out what this neurone is
saying by presenting different visual stimuli.
And you'll be able to see that during the course
of this video.
Well, you can hear the.
As I continue to continue to find.
What they're doing is they're going to the frontier now
where it's going to have to be a little crazy
for these things to be generated.
Reporter But this is a receptive field.
Getting into that.
And this particular unit is selected for the director most
of the time and.
Academic Congressional.
Important political orientation of the.
So that video just shows you you can use this
technique to see that these nerve cells in the visual
cortex are selective with the orientation, the motion direction of
of a visual stimulus.
And that's why they won the Nobel Prize.
By using this technique, you would not have been able
to determine that if you looked at the MRI signal,
which is something over hundreds of thousands of nerve cells.
You have to look at individual nerve cells to be
able to work that out.
Oops.
So the ultimate goal of all this new activity is
to control behaviour.
And I said that there were some limited circumstances in
which we could make these measurements in humans, including people,
for example, who are suffering from epilepsy, where we need
to measure from the brain to work out whereabouts the
surgical intervention could take place.
It also includes from people who in this case are
suffering from paraplegia, automatically paralysis.
Where the hope is that we recording the activity of
ourselves.
Cells will be able to help them regain function of
a robotic limb.
You have to effectively take what their brain, which is
still intact, is sending the signals that are sending and
use that to control something that we can then help
them move around the world.
In this particular case, these devices, which are often called
utile rays, which we have used in animals, I've never
used them in humans.
They were developed from being used in humans, have been
implanted in little part of the brain for the motor
cortex, which is important in generating movements.
And we'll get that in about two weeks time.
I just wanted to show you this video briefly because
it's incredibly evocative, like the Parkinson's video we watched the
other week and say to you, when we know how
we can interpret the signals and nerve cells, how powerful
that can be.
In this paper to people with Tetra and as two
people who were unable to move their arms or legs
in any functional, useful way, were able to control a
prosthetic or a robotic arm simply by thinking about the
movement of their own paralysed hand.
And they did that using the investigational BrainGate neural interface
system.
So they thought about using their own arm and hand
as though they were reaching out themselves with their own
limb.
And the robotic arm moved much the way their own
arm would have moved.
One of the longstanding questions not only in neuroscience but
in neuro rehabilitation, is whether the cells in the motor
cortex and other parts of the brain, whether they continue
to function the same way years after that original injury.
It is possible for people to use their thoughts to
control devices, either a computer or a robotic arm.
The way that happens is that we implant a tiny
sensor just about the size of a baby aspirin just
into the surface of the brain, and that sensor pick
up the electrical impulses from a bunch of neurones.
And each of those neurones are like radio broadcast towers
putting out impulses.
And when they get to the outside, the computer translated
converts the pattern of pulses into something that is a
command.
One of our participants was able to do something that
when all of a sort for the first time gave
us all pause.
She reached out with the robotic arm.
She thought about the use of her own hand.
She picked up that thermos of coffee, brought it close
to her, tilted it towards herself, and sipped coffee from
a straw.
That was the first time in nearly 15 years that
she had picked up anything and been able to drink
from it solely of her own volition.
There was a moment of true joy, true happiness.
I mean, it was beyond the fact that it was
an accomplishment.
I think an important advance in the entire field.
So I want to say that because actually that work
has, as we'll go into it next week, based on
the foundation of recording from these individual nerve cells or
small groups of them, in this case, maybe 50 or
100 nerve cells from the motor cortex.
And because we know what those individual nerve cells are
doing, and because we can record them simultaneously with these
kinds of devices, we can infer what the signal is
that she was trying to send her now arms at.
And now she's now disconnected from because she has paraplegia.
And because we were able to look at those individual
nerve cells, we can interpret that signal in ways that
is able to involve a prosthetic limb.
Now, when we when that device came out into the
market about 15 years ago, it seemed like a game
changer move from one electrode to 100 electrodes.
Well, recently and this is work from Nick Steinmetz and
colleagues from just across the road here.
We've developed new devices that can actually record from thousands
of neurones.
At the same time.
These are sometimes called neuro pixel devices.
We use these routinely.
Most people, many people use they'll now use these routinely.
We can record from multiple brain areas.
Sometimes the individual nerve cells, in each case a groups
of them within each of these brain areas.
And I don't want you to try and understand what's
on this line in terms of signals and measuring.
But the point is that we can start to interpret
how individual nerve cells across the brain are working together
to make cognitive decisions in these small animals lives which
share some of our brain capacity.
The other technique that you'll be exposed to over the
next few weeks is called calcium imaging.
I won't again, I won't take you through this in
any great detail.
It relies on the fact that every time an action
potentially is produced, I've talked to you about how sodium
ions come flexing into the cell.
There's also a little thing called calcium line to become
flexing in the cell.
And by designing particular molecules to sense the presence of
those calcium lines and make line signals dependent on how
many calcium lines are present, we can actually engage with
light the internal activity of so many cells at once.
We make injections or use animals of being transgenic engineered
to express these little proteins in many cells in the
cortex.
And we can use funky little microscopes or very large
ones, actually very expensive ones, to try and image the
activity of those cells in the brain without, in this
case, electrodes rather, using light to record the activity of
these nerve cells.
And this particular technique has a visual advantage with electrical
recordings.
We can't work out which cell we were recording from.
We know it was a single cell, but we don't
know which side it was.
But with calcium imaging, with imaging the brain, we actually
know which cell produced the activity.
We can take the brain down the animal after the
experiment, having killed the animal and then remove the brain.
And then we can process the brain tissue in particular
ways that allows us to look at the anatomical structure
of that brain circuit so we can take this functional
activity is activity recorded in, say, a hundred neurones in
a particular part of the brain and relate it to
the structural connections between those neurones and between those neurones
and the rest of the brain is not to bring
together.
Finally, how does individual nerve cells work together in a
circuit to provide cognitive function?
So that's why I'm interested in studying animals, because I
think that it's only by looking at the real structure,
the size structure of new activity in these circuits that
we'll understand the structure of cognition.
Now, I think they might disagree and say that animals
don't have interesting enough cognitive functions to allow us to
make much inference about humans.
But I hope that there will be some intersection between
those two things where we can actually find some cognitive
function in animals that we find interesting as humans, and
where we can understand the neural circuits that actually allow
that cognitive function.
So that's the span of the different techniques that we
might come across in this course.
The next election will be about how we can apply
some of these to, for example, rehabilitation.
And then after that, from next week onward, movies are
about specific systems, specific pathways for the brain.
We better wrap it up there as as was last
week, and you lead by this talk as well so
we can.
Thanks, everyone.
During.
Hi.
Hi.
Yeah.
Yeah, I know.
I worked that out this morning.
You think?
Yes, I'll do that.
Thanks for reminding me.
Like mom and.
Dad.
Oh, fantastic.
Love to see that.
Yes.
Good.
All right, well, I'll send those things to you today.
Anything else?
Not at the moment, I think.
Thanks.
I know at the amount of money.
Tucker Carlson get.
At the end of last week, they spoke briefly and
say how he's doing.
Yeah, I just want to know that we know from.
I can't blame the people.
Who are involved in the rescue issue.
In 1945.
Clinical Psychology.
I think having different people on the floor.
If you if you get.
I don't get the story.
I didn't.
I didn't.
Pretty promptly.
It's getting better.
I found last week because I'm given a lecture and
I live next to my two or three years.
Pacing was completely wrong.
Yeah, I know that.
She goes.
Is getting inadequate lecture fees?
Yes.
Down here.
Yeah.
I knew about luck.
Because the natural instinct is to end up that way.
Yeah.
Last week Hobson did it, and it's a week away
from doing it.
That's right.
There's no reason show will be the best.
But.
Oh, come into right now.
He's like, friend.
We think.
That there.
41.
Former.
Better.
It's been more than a game.
She.
Can.
But.
You.
Life.
Yeah.
So that was the thing that I think everybody.
That.
Great.
And you?
I want to point out.
Thank.
I didn't.
So.
So.
Okay.