File size: 53,150 Bytes
dae990d |
1 2 3 4 5 6 7 8 9 10 11 12 13 14 15 16 17 18 19 20 21 22 23 24 25 26 27 28 29 30 31 32 33 34 35 36 37 38 39 40 41 42 43 44 45 46 47 48 49 50 51 52 53 54 55 56 57 58 59 60 61 62 63 64 65 66 67 68 69 70 71 72 73 74 75 76 77 78 79 80 81 82 83 84 85 86 87 88 89 90 91 92 93 94 95 96 97 98 99 100 101 102 103 104 105 106 107 108 109 110 111 112 113 114 115 116 117 118 119 120 121 122 123 124 125 126 127 128 129 130 131 132 133 134 135 136 137 138 139 140 141 142 143 144 145 146 147 148 149 150 151 152 153 154 155 156 157 158 159 160 161 162 163 164 165 166 167 168 169 170 171 172 173 174 175 176 177 178 179 180 181 182 183 184 185 186 187 188 189 190 191 192 193 194 195 196 197 198 199 200 201 202 203 204 205 206 207 208 209 210 211 212 213 214 215 216 217 218 219 220 221 222 223 224 225 226 227 228 229 230 231 232 233 234 235 236 237 238 239 240 241 242 243 244 245 246 247 248 249 250 251 252 253 254 255 256 257 258 259 260 261 262 263 264 265 266 267 268 269 270 271 272 273 274 275 276 277 278 279 280 281 282 283 284 285 286 287 288 289 290 291 292 293 294 295 296 297 298 299 300 301 302 303 304 305 306 307 308 309 310 311 312 313 314 315 316 317 318 319 320 321 322 323 324 325 326 327 328 329 330 331 332 333 334 335 336 337 338 339 340 341 342 343 344 345 346 347 348 349 350 351 352 353 354 355 356 357 358 359 360 361 362 363 364 365 366 367 368 369 370 371 372 373 374 375 376 377 378 379 380 381 382 383 384 385 386 387 388 389 390 391 392 393 394 395 396 397 398 399 400 401 402 403 404 405 406 407 408 409 410 411 412 413 414 415 416 417 418 419 420 421 422 423 424 425 426 427 428 429 430 431 432 433 434 435 436 437 438 439 440 441 442 443 444 445 446 447 448 449 450 451 452 453 454 455 456 457 458 459 460 461 462 463 464 465 466 467 468 469 470 471 472 473 474 475 476 477 478 479 480 481 482 483 484 485 486 487 488 489 490 491 492 493 494 495 496 497 498 499 500 501 502 503 504 505 506 507 508 509 510 511 512 513 514 515 516 517 518 519 520 521 522 523 524 525 526 527 528 529 530 531 532 533 534 535 536 537 538 539 540 541 542 543 544 545 546 547 548 549 550 551 552 553 554 555 556 557 558 559 560 561 562 563 564 565 566 567 568 569 570 571 572 573 574 575 576 577 578 579 580 581 582 583 584 585 586 587 588 589 590 591 592 593 594 595 596 597 598 599 600 601 602 603 604 605 606 607 608 609 610 611 612 613 614 615 616 617 618 619 620 621 622 623 624 625 626 627 628 629 630 631 632 633 634 635 636 637 638 639 640 641 642 643 644 645 646 647 648 649 650 651 652 653 654 655 656 657 658 659 660 661 662 663 664 665 666 667 668 669 670 671 672 673 674 675 676 677 678 679 680 681 682 683 684 685 686 687 688 689 690 691 692 693 694 695 696 697 698 699 700 701 702 703 704 705 706 707 708 709 710 711 712 713 714 715 716 717 718 719 720 721 722 723 724 725 726 727 728 729 730 731 732 733 734 735 736 737 738 739 740 741 742 743 744 745 746 747 748 749 750 751 752 753 754 755 756 757 758 759 760 761 762 763 764 765 766 767 768 769 770 771 772 773 774 775 776 777 778 779 780 781 782 783 784 785 786 787 788 789 790 791 792 793 794 795 796 797 798 799 800 801 802 803 804 805 806 807 808 809 810 811 812 813 814 815 816 817 818 819 820 821 822 823 824 825 826 827 828 829 830 831 832 833 834 835 836 837 838 839 840 841 842 843 844 845 846 847 848 849 850 851 852 853 854 855 856 857 858 859 860 861 862 863 864 865 866 867 868 869 870 871 872 873 874 875 876 877 878 879 880 881 882 883 884 885 886 887 888 889 890 891 892 893 894 895 896 897 898 899 900 901 902 903 904 905 906 907 908 909 910 911 912 913 914 915 916 917 918 919 920 921 922 923 924 925 926 927 928 929 930 931 932 933 934 935 936 937 938 939 940 941 942 943 944 945 946 947 948 949 950 951 952 953 954 955 956 957 958 959 960 961 962 963 964 965 966 967 968 969 970 971 972 973 974 975 976 977 978 979 980 981 982 983 984 985 986 987 988 989 990 991 992 993 994 995 996 997 998 999 1000 1001 1002 1003 1004 1005 1006 1007 1008 1009 1010 1011 1012 1013 1014 1015 1016 1017 1018 1019 1020 1021 1022 1023 1024 1025 1026 1027 1028 1029 1030 1031 1032 1033 1034 1035 1036 1037 1038 1039 1040 1041 1042 1043 1044 1045 1046 1047 1048 1049 1050 1051 1052 1053 1054 1055 1056 1057 1058 1059 1060 1061 1062 1063 1064 1065 1066 1067 1068 1069 1070 1071 1072 1073 1074 1075 1076 1077 1078 1079 1080 1081 1082 1083 1084 1085 1086 1087 1088 1089 1090 1091 1092 1093 1094 1095 1096 1097 1098 1099 1100 1101 1102 1103 1104 1105 1106 1107 1108 1109 1110 1111 1112 1113 1114 1115 1116 1117 1118 1119 1120 1121 1122 1123 1124 1125 1126 1127 1128 1129 1130 1131 1132 1133 1134 1135 1136 1137 1138 1139 1140 1141 1142 1143 1144 1145 1146 1147 1148 1149 1150 1151 1152 1153 1154 1155 1156 1157 1158 1159 1160 1161 1162 1163 1164 1165 1166 1167 1168 1169 1170 1171 1172 1173 1174 1175 1176 1177 1178 1179 1180 1181 1182 1183 1184 1185 1186 1187 1188 1189 1190 1191 1192 1193 1194 1195 1196 1197 1198 1199 1200 1201 1202 1203 1204 1205 1206 1207 1208 1209 1210 1211 1212 1213 1214 1215 1216 1217 1218 1219 1220 1221 1222 1223 1224 1225 1226 1227 1228 1229 1230 1231 1232 1233 1234 1235 1236 1237 1238 1239 1240 1241 1242 1243 1244 1245 1246 1247 1248 1249 1250 1251 1252 1253 1254 1255 1256 1257 1258 1259 1260 1261 1262 1263 1264 1265 1266 1267 1268 1269 1270 1271 1272 1273 1274 1275 1276 1277 1278 1279 1280 1281 1282 1283 1284 1285 1286 1287 1288 1289 1290 1291 1292 1293 1294 1295 1296 1297 1298 1299 1300 1301 1302 1303 1304 1305 1306 1307 1308 1309 1310 1311 1312 1313 1314 1315 1316 1317 1318 1319 1320 1321 1322 1323 1324 1325 1326 1327 1328 1329 1330 1331 1332 1333 1334 1335 1336 1337 1338 1339 1340 1341 1342 1343 1344 1345 1346 1347 1348 1349 1350 1351 1352 1353 1354 1355 1356 1357 1358 1359 1360 1361 1362 1363 1364 1365 1366 1367 1368 1369 1370 1371 1372 1373 1374 1375 1376 1377 1378 1379 1380 1381 1382 1383 1384 1385 1386 1387 1388 1389 1390 1391 1392 1393 1394 1395 1396 1397 1398 1399 1400 1401 1402 1403 1404 1405 1406 1407 1408 1409 1410 1411 1412 1413 1414 1415 1416 1417 1418 1419 1420 1421 1422 1423 1424 1425 1426 1427 1428 1429 1430 1431 1432 1433 1434 1435 1436 1437 1438 1439 1440 1441 1442 1443 1444 1445 1446 1447 1448 1449 1450 1451 1452 1453 1454 1455 1456 1457 1458 1459 1460 1461 1462 1463 1464 1465 1466 1467 1468 1469 1470 1471 1472 1473 1474 1475 1476 1477 1478 1479 1480 1481 1482 1483 1484 1485 1486 1487 1488 1489 1490 1491 1492 1493 1494 1495 1496 1497 1498 1499 1500 1501 1502 1503 1504 1505 1506 1507 1508 1509 1510 1511 1512 1513 1514 1515 1516 1517 1518 1519 1520 1521 1522 1523 1524 1525 1526 1527 1528 1529 1530 1531 1532 1533 1534 1535 1536 1537 1538 1539 1540 1541 1542 1543 1544 1545 1546 1547 1548 1549 1550 1551 1552 1553 1554 1555 1556 1557 1558 1559 1560 1561 1562 1563 1564 1565 1566 1567 1568 1569 1570 1571 1572 1573 1574 1575 1576 1577 1578 1579 1580 1581 1582 1583 1584 1585 1586 1587 1588 1589 1590 1591 1592 1593 1594 1595 1596 1597 1598 1599 1600 1601 1602 1603 1604 1605 1606 1607 1608 1609 1610 1611 1612 1613 1614 1615 1616 1617 1618 1619 1620 1621 1622 1623 1624 1625 1626 1627 1628 1629 1630 1631 1632 1633 1634 1635 1636 1637 1638 1639 1640 1641 1642 1643 1644 1645 1646 1647 1648 1649 1650 1651 1652 1653 1654 1655 1656 1657 1658 1659 1660 1661 1662 1663 1664 1665 1666 1667 1668 1669 1670 1671 1672 1673 1674 1675 1676 1677 1678 1679 1680 1681 1682 1683 1684 1685 1686 1687 1688 1689 1690 1691 1692 1693 1694 1695 1696 1697 1698 1699 1700 1701 1702 1703 1704 1705 1706 1707 1708 1709 1710 1711 1712 1713 1714 1715 1716 1717 1718 1719 1720 1721 1722 1723 1724 1725 1726 1727 1728 1729 1730 1731 1732 1733 1734 1735 1736 1737 1738 1739 1740 1741 1742 1743 1744 1745 1746 1747 1748 1749 1750 1751 1752 1753 1754 1755 1756 1757 1758 1759 1760 1761 1762 1763 1764 1765 1766 1767 1768 1769 1770 1771 1772 1773 1774 1775 1776 1777 1778 1779 1780 1781 1782 1783 1784 1785 1786 1787 1788 1789 1790 1791 1792 1793 1794 1795 1796 1797 1798 1799 1800 1801 1802 1803 1804 1805 1806 1807 1808 1809 1810 1811 1812 1813 1814 1815 1816 1817 1818 1819 1820 1821 1822 1823 1824 1825 1826 1827 1828 1829 1830 1831 1832 1833 1834 1835 1836 1837 1838 1839 1840 1841 1842 1843 1844 1845 1846 1847 1848 1849 1850 1851 1852 1853 1854 1855 1856 1857 1858 1859 1860 1861 1862 1863 1864 1865 1866 1867 1868 1869 1870 1871 1872 1873 1874 1875 1876 1877 1878 1879 1880 1881 1882 1883 1884 1885 1886 1887 1888 1889 1890 1891 1892 1893 1894 1895 1896 1897 1898 1899 1900 1901 1902 1903 1904 1905 1906 1907 1908 1909 1910 1911 1912 1913 1914 1915 1916 1917 1918 1919 1920 1921 1922 1923 1924 1925 1926 1927 1928 1929 1930 1931 1932 1933 1934 1935 1936 1937 1938 1939 1940 1941 1942 1943 1944 1945 1946 1947 1948 1949 1950 1951 1952 1953 1954 1955 1956 1957 1958 1959 1960 1961 1962 1963 1964 1965 1966 1967 1968 1969 1970 1971 1972 1973 1974 1975 1976 1977 1978 1979 1980 1981 1982 1983 1984 1985 1986 1987 1988 1989 1990 1991 1992 1993 1994 1995 1996 1997 1998 1999 2000 2001 2002 2003 2004 2005 2006 2007 2008 2009 2010 2011 2012 2013 2014 2015 2016 2017 2018 2019 2020 2021 2022 2023 2024 2025 2026 2027 2028 2029 2030 2031 2032 2033 2034 2035 2036 2037 2038 2039 2040 2041 2042 2043 2044 2045 2046 2047 2048 2049 2050 2051 2052 2053 2054 2055 2056 2057 2058 2059 2060 2061 2062 2063 2064 2065 2066 2067 2068 2069 2070 2071 2072 2073 2074 2075 2076 2077 2078 2079 2080 2081 2082 2083 2084 2085 2086 2087 2088 2089 2090 2091 2092 2093 2094 2095 2096 2097 2098 2099 2100 2101 2102 2103 2104 2105 2106 2107 2108 2109 2110 2111 2112 2113 2114 2115 2116 2117 2118 2119 2120 2121 2122 2123 2124 2125 2126 2127 2128 2129 2130 2131 2132 2133 2134 2135 2136 2137 2138 2139 2140 2141 2142 2143 2144 2145 2146 2147 2148 2149 2150 2151 2152 2153 2154 2155 2156 2157 2158 2159 2160 2161 2162 2163 2164 2165 2166 2167 2168 2169 2170 2171 2172 2173 2174 2175 2176 2177 2178 2179 2180 2181 2182 2183 2184 2185 2186 2187 2188 2189 2190 2191 2192 2193 2194 2195 2196 2197 2198 2199 2200 2201 2202 2203 2204 2205 2206 2207 2208 2209 2210 2211 2212 2213 2214 2215 2216 2217 2218 2219 2220 2221 2222 2223 2224 2225 2226 2227 2228 2229 2230 2231 2232 2233 2234 2235 2236 2237 2238 2239 2240 2241 2242 2243 2244 2245 2246 2247 2248 2249 2250 2251 2252 2253 2254 2255 2256 2257 2258 2259 2260 2261 2262 2263 2264 2265 2266 2267 2268 2269 2270 2271 2272 2273 2274 2275 2276 2277 2278 2279 2280 2281 2282 2283 2284 2285 2286 2287 2288 2289 2290 2291 2292 2293 2294 2295 2296 2297 2298 2299 2300 2301 2302 2303 2304 2305 2306 2307 2308 2309 2310 2311 2312 2313 2314 2315 2316 2317 2318 2319 2320 2321 2322 2323 2324 2325 2326 2327 2328 2329 2330 2331 2332 2333 2334 2335 2336 2337 2338 2339 2340 2341 2342 2343 2344 2345 2346 2347 2348 2349 2350 2351 2352 2353 2354 2355 2356 2357 2358 2359 2360 2361 2362 2363 2364 2365 2366 2367 2368 2369 2370 2371 2372 2373 2374 2375 2376 2377 2378 2379 2380 2381 2382 2383 2384 2385 2386 2387 2388 2389 |
Just. This is Christopher in gold XO d2. It is eight degrees in my theatre at the moment. I'm not sure if you can do anything about that, but I do think that certainly if we work the incredible work rate in the. Thank you for joining me. It's a I grew up in a very home country, Australia, and I don't think I saw snow until I was in my mid-twenties. So for me, it's still awe inspiring to walk through a white snow field. So I hope you enjoyed your journey. And today I am in my place. But yeah, I'm here. This is behaviourism. Yeah, I. My brain is frozen. I just. I hope that's not on, but it's not going to help us in the next hour, I'm afraid. So thank you for joining me today. It's really nice to see you all again. I'm going to be talking about neurological disorders. It's a little bit I mean, I have mixed feelings about giving this lecture. I always feel a little bit depressed. But this year I tried to be a to alleviate my depression a little bit by introducing a couple of slides illustrating some very recent advances in treating these disorders, which give me hope at least that we're entering a period where we might be able to take these on and generate cures, or at least treatments for people. Sorry, my brain has actually frozen, so I'll try and get through this if I put my jacket on. Well, the problem is if I put my jacket on, it rustles the volume. But I see. Why a neurological disorder is so problematic. I think what you've learned so far in the course should give you a bit of a cue to answering this. I think the major thing, the first major things, that nerve cells are not self-renewing, but you're basically born with the same number of nerve cells you end up with. And you just lose them. There's a few that are born maybe in the hippocampus and the old factory lobe, but most of the neurones that you have on you now are ones that you were born with. And that means that if one of them dies, you're not going to get replaced. The second thing is that neurones are excitable cells. As we've talked a lot. I like action potentials. I do a lot of work to try and generate these action potentials and I can actually be overexcited or it's a bit of a loose term, but I can this generating this activity can lead to disorder within the cells. So those cells, if they get overexcited, can actually trigger apoptosis. That is cell death. Generating all this activity requires a lot of energy. It requires a very specific way of getting and be very, I think, by the way, in the brain of getting the energy to the right part of the brain. If we disorder that blood supply, energy supply as we go through time. Those neurones also form what we will call what we have called recurrent circuit. That is circuits where the axons from one cell connect to the dendrite to another cell and vice versa and so forth. These are very exquisite connections that are both partly genetically predetermined, but a lot of which is set up by experience early in life and in later life. Recapitulating those networks generated in those networks again, or putting a neurone in that network and asking it to replace the function of a new one that's already there. That's effectively impossible. You would have to recapitulate the experience that those neurones have had to be able to regenerate that network. So these for all these reasons, it's very difficult when the brain starts to be disordered, so start to die. Treating those disorders is really, really hard. So in this lecture, I just want to take you through a couple of different disorders that we know something about. And I've chosen these very carefully because there's many disorders that we do not know much about at all. In talking about these, I will be talking about disorders are called intrinsic rather than extrinsic. Extrinsic ones are ones like, for example, tumours being developing in the brain. By the way, since nerve cells and not the actual part of the tumour because they don't replicate in the same ways. Extreme disorders also include traumatic brain injury. For example, if you have a car accident or if you get concussed many times as a footballer. And those kinds of disorders are not a consideration here because they're really just a result, not just the result of things that the brain can't do much about. So we want to talk a little bit about intrinsic one, things that arise because of the particular structure of the brain, because of the way the brain works. I won't be talking about developmental brain abnormalities in this particular lecture. What I will be talking about is degenerative disorders, looking at Alzheimer's disease in particular, and also epileptic seizures. I'd like to start by talking about seizures. Seizures? Does anyone know much about seizures at all? You come across them. You may have seen someone having a seizure at some point in time. This is quite distressing experience. I find it's a this is a cause that we'll find out in. The second is caused by overly synchronous brain activity, by synchronous means that the neurones, the nerve cells are all firing at the same time. We'll see what the effect of that is in a second. About 3% of all people suffer from epilepsy at some time in life, most commonly in childhood or older age. I think it's not unexpected because that's the time at which the brain is changing most and therefore the balance that is required to prevent seizures, it's most likely to be disordered. At least 30% of athletic facilities have some known genetic basis, whereas about 25% have what we call acquired antecedents that, for example, rheumatic brain injuries. So professor of epilepsy, a friend of mine, has suffered from epilepsy since he had a bike accident 20 years ago, and he's under treatment for it. But it's still something that affects his life. You know that that doesn't add up 100%. And the other 50% of their bounce that the causes are known either because there was inadequate record taking at the time or because they have a complex, multifactorial basis. We know a lot about seizures since the development of electroencephalogram, ability to measure the activity of the brain from the scalp. On the left hand side to see what would look like a normal EEG. And is the traces in the brain due to the electronic wiggle a little bit that they're not overly wiggly and they're not overly synchronised between different electrodes. Two types of seizures as shown on the right here. The generalised seizure here, which would be part of a grand mal seizure. It means that the activity in the brain is synchronised, basically causing time brain. And you can see that here because the big wiggles on the on the electron system, grandma all happen at the same time. That means that neurones in each part of the brain that is generating these EEG signals are firing together at the same time in big discharge bursts of discharge synchronising their activity. Of more interest from a psychological perspective really is the partial seizures, which is shown in the middle here. And that is where some parts of the brain start to synchronise and other bits seem to be untarnished. We'll be going into that in a second. We've touched a little bit on brain rhythms over the course of these lectures. Rhythms are effectively a natural part of any biological system. Can anyone tell me one rhythm you can think of? Circadian rhythm. That's a daily rhythm in trained by light, but actually set by pacemaker cells or cells within this whole area of the brain. Probably the super charismatic nucleus whose activity varies with over about a 25 hour cycle. I think if you take if you if you take someone and put them in or if you take an animal and put them in the constant dark, you'll see they still have a circadian rhythm, but it's not quite locked to the 24 hour clock. That's that locking comes from exposure to light, which resets that pacemaker. Is there any other rhythms that you can think of? What am I doing right now? Well, that's a good one as well. But that's not a that's actually set by within within the heart pacemaker activity in the heart. What am I doing? What are most of us all doing? We're all breathing, right? The breathing is also a rhythm that's set by the pacemaker cells and a loop part of the brainstem. I do fact reports, and if I remember rightly, these activity pulsates, of course, can be modulated. They can be fast and be slow. Hold your breath. It normally would act to inflate the diaphragm. These kinds of brain rhythms are those are those are the properties really of individual cells, or at least there's cycles or rhythms within those individual cells. The kind of rhythms that get disordered in seizures, though, are not really properties of individual cells, but properties of networks of cells. So what happens is that when you have excitatory cells talking to each other and then feeding back to each other, and if you have inhibitory cells helping set that. These cells. When you fire an action, potentially send it to another one and that fires an action potential and sends it back to you. That creates a loop and that creates a natural rhythm from that circuit. And so depending on the structure of the circuit, you get different archetypal rhythms emerging. So this slide here shows you three of the classic rhythms, the alpha rhythm, the spindle rhythm and the ripple. They found, or at least are most prominent in particular brain regions, and they are the result of particular pathways for communication between brain regions. So, for example, the rhythm, which is most prominent actually in the visual cortex at the back of the brain, is primarily a cortical rhythm. It's about 12 hertz. The really prominent rhythm. It's much stronger when your eyes are closed, when your eyes are open. A spindle rhythm is about the interaction between the thalamus and the cortex. So neurones in the thalamus send signals to the cortex to send signals back to the thalamus. And this loop itself, which is responsible for a large part of slow wave sleep, also generates what's called spindle rhythms. And then these ripple rhythms are the communication between cells within the hippocampus, which generate a very sharp, very fast rhythm. You can see the timescale here is 100 milliseconds as opposed to one second here. So this slide also shows this seems to show that these rhythms are not just a product of something found in humans. They're actually found in almost all species that have been studied. In fact, all species that have been studied. You can identify these rhythms if you look in the right place and search hard enough. So in humans, in non-human primates, in dogs, cats, bats, rabbits, rodents. In other words, you can find these are rhythms. And it is quite striking that if you look at the frequency, that is the number of cycles per second that these rhythms take place over. As I said, Alpha was about 12 hertz or 12 times per second. So 12 of these little squiggles per second and the ripple is about 250 hertz. If you look across a large range of species, you find that the rhythms in each of those species are about the same frequency, even though their brains can vary in orders of magnitude of number of neurones and all of the magnitude and numbers of signs. And that suggests to us that these rhythms are important for brain function. What they do is not so clear. So there's many hypotheses out there, for example, in camera rhythms, very fast rhythms, about 30 or 50 hertz are important for consciousness and for propagation of signals between cortical areas. Yet there is very little hard evidence that they are necessary for that those activities. So, yes. You can't. I think it's just been not studied there. I'm not sure if it's not present. I'd be willing to know. I haven't studied all of those animals. It's you do have to report them in particular ways and in particular circumstances. For example, slow way rhythms or any form during sleep. And so you have to be recording from an animal during sleep. So I'm not quite sure if they're missing on this one a bit older. So if they're missing because they're absent or missing just because I haven't studied, it's a good question. So these rhythms of what is disordered and what a what prominent about seizure disorders. This slide just simply classifies the general types of seizures. We're not going to be talking about the grand mal seizures, these massive changes to the brain function. However, what I would like to go through a little bit is partial seizures, because they tell they're explicit explicable going from the structure of the brain. Possible seizures can be either simple or complex. Complex partial seizures results in a second often involved impairment or loss of consciousness, or a simple partial seizures. A person who suffering a seizure suffers no loss of consciousness during the. This slide, which is a little bit busy here, but you can look at in more detail when you feel like it. On the top left is a description of some of the features of a complex partial seizure. On the bottom right, a simple partial seizure. Partial seizure complex Partial seizures often start in the temporal lobe or in the frontal cortex. And maybe that's why they're actually associated often with a loss of consciousness, or at least an impairment of consciousness. The squiggles over here show that the frontal and temple ones are undergoing large amplified rhythms, whereas the simple ones are not so prominent. In this process. He's a simple, partial Caesar here, however, usually involving the sensory with the motor cortices. You can see here that over a typical and a little bit the motor cortex, some big spikes, but not so much in the frontal and temporal lobes. So simple procedures often involve motor disturbances, for example, tremor that emerge because your motor cortex is being disordered and cortex is oscillating in a rhythmic fashion. So your muscles will be oscillating in a rhythmic fashion. And simple procedures are quite interesting because we can because someone is conscious and because they're not too problematic, we can actually track their progression through the cortical structures. So, for example, I really like this description, and I should know, by the way, that John Huling Jackson, who you go introduce you to back in the first lecture I think was based in Queens where when he made these discoveries was the one who proposed that the progression of simple partial seizures reflected the structure of the brain organisation. It's a nice description of what might be a typical seizure. Is on his right foot, began to shake. Now a lower leg was shaking than a proper leg as well. With horrified fascination, she felt her body begin to shake and reason with her leg. Is shaking slowed and then finally stopped. This is a real case. A CT scan showed a small white circled spot between the frontal lobes above the corpus place and a small tumour. This woman had a simple procedure that actually progressed to a complex seizure. Sometime later. So this little tumour that emerged in the brain was up here between the two lobes at the very top of the motor cortex. If you remember back to the monthlies that we looked at when we talked about sensorimotor cortices, the progression of disease or progression of the disturbances make sense from the structure that most of us. A foot is represented at the top of the brain, near the open close, and the tongue, the face and head emphasised. So is the progression of that seizure of that epileptic form activity move across the cortical surface? So the shaking in different parts of the body reflected that among the organisation. This makes sense for everyone. This simple partial CS is effectively a travelling wave starting from a particular point and moving across the surface of the cortex. Gradually recruiting different parts of the body that represent or control the muscles in particular parts of the body. Some, as I mentioned before, some seizures seem to be familial, inherited. I've learned quite a lot about them by studying rare cases of one of the psychotic twins and looking at the seizures that they sometimes have. This is one famous example. This is a pair of Monozygotic twins, Constance and Catherine, who have absence epilepsy in this case. What I want you to take away from this is electroencephalogram recordings taken from Constance and Catherine, I think at different times. Or you should notice the structure of the epileptic form discharges that are happening on these EEG channels are remarkably similar across the two twins. So over here, these these structures, these regions are very similar over here and so forth. So the structure of the seizures that they're having seems to be very similar, seem to have the genetic basis should be identifiable and seem to control the seizures that they're having. Indeed, if we actually look across a large number of cohort studies and trends, we find a large piecewise concordance between those two things. In generalised epilepsies, about 80% of the variance is explained and even in focal epilepsies, about 40% of the variance is explained. This case was concordance goes down substantially if you look at guys iconic twins rather than monozygotic twins. So there's something particular about some of the genes in these individuals that is disrupted. And remember, back to the first couple of lectures we had when we talked about the presence of ion channels in the membranes of nerve cells, it should make sense. A lot of these disorders are the disorder of specific kinds of ion channels that are important in regulating neuronal excitability, particularly sodium channels and potassium channels. These channels get disordered and therefore the normal structure of new activity is disrupted and the cells or the circuits that they are part of become susceptible to the possibility of being pushed into regimes where they become epileptic form. Remember, this letter form activity is not just an individual, so rhythmically discharging its spiking activity across a network of nerve cells, hundreds of thousands of cells, it suddenly becomes entrained makes this reverberant circuit of activity. So normally the activity of really cold, only the activity of the brain is sufficient to suppress the epileptic form discharge. But in people with these channel up with these, as they called that activity, is that the normal ways of controlling these these activity, stopping neurones from getting hyper excitable is disrupted and these people are therefore susceptible to seizures. She'll also say that you can actually many people are also sensitive to what's called photosensitive epilepsy. You're probably all aware of those trigger warnings that come up often between newspaper. News reports on the TV saying there will be flash photography in the upcoming segment. And the reason those trigger warnings are there is because if you have sensitive epilepsy, you can be susceptible. You can be induced to have a seizure response seizure anyway by the presence of that flickering light from the flash photography. It's not exactly clear how or why this this works, why the light entering the eye in trains, rhythms in the brain, therefore causes his epileptic seizures. But about 3% of people suffering from epileptic seizures have this sort of sensitive epilepsy, generally triggered by lights of flicker and about 15 to 25 frames per second. Close to the aphorism that we mentioned over visual cortex, and maybe that's got something to do with it. We just don't know. And can be triggered by ceiling fan, strobe strobe lights, etc. for anything that makes this rhythmic on 15 to 20 times per second. Visual image. The following statement I leave you because I don't actually really understand. I wrote it down because I remember this happening. But who hears what's spoken on? Are too ashamed to admit? Or are you just too old to have done that? Apparently. Apparently, I've never worked for one in my life. The 38 episode broadcast on 64th December 1997, includes a scene where Ash, Ashes and his friends need to go inside the pokey for whatever that is through any device. I remember when it happened, I remember it being a big news story across the world, presumably because a lot of people watching Pokemon on that stage, I suppose very few of you actually born then. But what happened in this thing was the repetitive red, the blue flicker about five times per second, which ash was presumably seen and which was transmitted through the TV screens and induced about 600 to 700 people to have to go to hospital for photosensitive epilepsy in Japan at the time. So this is the reason that we have these trigger warnings when we see flash photography and the people making TV and movies now much more aware of the possibility for sensitive epilepsy. I want to show you some recent progress actually from Queen Square. Again, just as John Houston's Jackson, some from whatever, years ago. This is work from a group of people at a very large epilepsy research group at the Queen Square Institute of Neurology. I think this is a really interesting and important innovation. I'll take you through. It came out earlier this year. It also speaks a little bit to some of the noise that you've picked up over the last few weeks. The underlying thought from these researchers was the following. Epileptic activity is this increase in discharge above the normal rate in nerve cells? It follows that that increase in discharge triggers some processes, interestingly, that we might be able to tap into. In particular, this thing called IED or immediate. Early gene is something we've known for about 25 years now that when neurones become more active than normal, that triggers this expression of this thing called IED. We don't know exactly what it's doing, but we know it gets expressed. And these researchers have the idea. All right, We know that this stuff only gets expressed in these regimes of very heightened activity. So if we can introduce a construct into the cells that are sensitive to the expression of those immediate early genes, we might be able to turn down the activity in the cells that have been over activated. And so they introduced what's called a form of potassium channel that actually decreases neuronal excitability and whose expression expression of this protein was linked to the expression of this immediate early gene. So when an epileptic discharge starts, the hypothesis is that when they discharge start, that increases neural activity, which in turn drives the production of this immediate early gene. That immediate early gene production in turn drives the production of this novel protein that they've been able to introduce into the cells by means of a virus is otherwise not destructive to the cells. And then that that expression of that protein in turn will drive down the activity and stop the seizure. That was a hypothesis they wanted to explore. And the work that I published earlier this year suggests that this is true and might be a mechanism for actually treating fungal epilepsies, at least in some patients. To do this, they induced in a mouse model to test this hypothesis because epilepsy is a product of brain networks and not easily reproduced and induced, although they did do work in vivo before doing this in vivo working mouse they induced. There's a standard model of generating epilepsy in mice. In mice don't suffer too much from this. They do get seizures, but this doesn't seem to cause much pain or distress. After after these seizures were induced, they then measured the electrical activity by an electrical program electrode placed on the brain. And they injected these viruses into the parts of the brain where the seizures were occurring. So if they inject this far in the brain, they can then compare the reduction of seizures in the mice that have been treated with the control virus or this virus that produces this particular protein. And sure enough, they find it was in the control virus conditions. Mice continued to have seizures as each of these little batches shows over the course of several weeks at a time of a seizure. They're recording continuously, by the way, from the animals. They find that in these animals treated with the active form of the virus, that those ones are stopping the seizures. So this opens up the possibility that we might be able to inject into people who are having intractable epileptic seizures, at least focal seizures. We start from particular part of the brain. We can inject into that part of the brain some virus that expresses a protein like this, but then in turn allows that part of the brain to effectively control production of epileptic discharges. I showed. In other words, that I'm not showing here, they showed that the presence of this virus in doesn't interfere with normal behaviour, but only really with epileptic seizures. They're really important and encouraging evidence because people with intractable epilepsy at the moment, the sole way of trying to address the seizures is to try and excise from the brain a little bit of brain tissue to generate the seizure. So instead of taking out that piece of brain tissue, the hope is that we can use a virus that's otherwise safe but allows the neurones to suppress the epileptic discharge. A really important advance from my colleagues over Queensway. So remember with a simple partial seizures which spread from a part of the brain down to another, therefore causing trembling and different things that focal because they start at a particular point and then they spread like a travelling wave across the brain, as opposed to generalised epilepsy, which is a large. Synchronisation of brain activity across the entire brain. So this focus on start at particular point and spread. And because I started at the point you can actually introduce a virus at that particular point I should have said that those focal partial seizures, they start from a reproducible place in the person's brain. It's not like they start from different places on different seasons. They start from the same place each time and spread across the brain from that same place. So because the you can then introduce this virus and try to. See this as an end or partial. Or is are there like an off? I don't think there are. I think they have to be focal otherwise that generalised. I don't know whether or not the size of that focus could be quite variable. I have a friend who studies epilepsy epileptic patients in Australia. He actually a lot of people, as you may know, a lot of people that have epilepsy have aura associated with a preceding the epileptic discharge. And often this aura has structure a bit like the migraine has associated with migraine has structure and can actually because of that, people know when they're about to have or at least some time before they're about to have an electrical seizure. And my friend has been studying those people by putting them in a brain scanner during the seizure, actually. These are at least four seizures, partial seizures, which don't have massive consequences for the individuals. And you can see the progression of this activity in the brain during during scanning. Okay, So that's I just want to take through the seizures that clear result really of probably of mainly of channel opposites all these disorders of ion channels that regulate the normal excitability of neurones. The hope is that we can try and treat these by effectively resurrecting that normal control of those ion channels. Seizures are fairly rare and we're going to make a lot of progress in trying to deal with them. Other disorders were made much less progress, and one of the most prominent is Alzheimer's disease, a form of dementia which is increasingly prevalent. So there's actually many different types of dementias. My grandmother had Alzheimer's disease. Many people's grandparents, I suspect, or even parents will be suffering from Alzheimer's disease or related dementia. Alzheimer's disease is by far the largest, most common occurrence. How? How well it can be distinguished from other types of dementia is still a matter of debate. These dementias have common causes. It's still quite. Another common form of dementia is vascular dementia. That's where three years old strokes, which then allow a little bit of blood in parts of the brain, turn triggers the brain to start self-destruction basically in that area. You can get dementia from having many of these multiple little strokes, which then have an impact on brain function. There's also much less common forms of dementia which have been important, like the one those iconic twins with seizure disorders. Those common those rare forms of dementia have been very important to understanding the potential mechanisms of dementia, because we can look at some of the genetic contributions of those. This includes frontotemporal dementia, Pick's disease course for Jakob Disease, Huntington's disease, Parkinson's dementia and Lewy body disease. Many of these diseases have other effects as well. Dementia is a part of the condition, not necessarily the entire condition. I got this line from This is the last World Report from the World from the Outside Disease Foundation. This just goes to show you the kind of prevalence of Alzheimer's disease now and predicted prevalence in the future. About one person around the world becomes diagnosed with dementia every 3 seconds. In 2015, there are already 50 million people in the world who suffering from dementia is projected to be triple that in 2050. It's a huge we spend a large amount of resources that we produce treating people with dementia. And it's a worldwide phenomenon in particular as life expectancy increases in the global south. Dementia is becoming more and more prevalent there as well and has to date being primarily a ritual disease because life expectancy is longer in those parts of the world and Alzheimer's disease or other dimensions. And we strike older people. I think the current ratio is something like 20% of people over the age of 18 will have Alzheimer's disease or dementia before they die. The number is much less for people over 65. So as as life expectancy increases, as more and more people live over the age of 80, respect dementia, profound increase around the world. It's hard for those of us who don't have dementia to gain some insight into what it feels like, what it is like to suffer from dementia. I find this William Autumn Olin's. Ought to be at least one attempted insight into that. He was diagnosed with the Alzheimer's disease in 1995. He actually lived a bit longer than most people who suffer from Alzheimer's disease. And he was able as an artist to try and describe some of the. Changes in his cognition as he developed Alzheimer's disease, or at least in the first few years. This is the first portrait he made when he was diagnosed, actually a man untethered, someone lost in the world. As dementia progressed, as his disorder progressed, his his art takes on particular forms. You can see that he's a very accomplished artist, very capable of generating self-portraits. Back in 1996, in this case, when he's only a year or so after the diagnosis. What I hope you notice is that the structure of these images changes substantially over the next five years. There's a lot of death, lots of structure. There's a changing affect. There's capacity to see and to feel is changing fairly quickly over these several years. And really having a disorder of both his representation of the world, but also his internal representation of himself. Alzheimer's disease is named after Alois Alzheimer, who was an Austrian neuropathologist at the turn of the last century. He made his discoveries by studying data. Peter Peter, who was one of the unfortunate people who suffered early onset Alzheimer's disease. She was only about 50 when she was taken to his unit by her husband, who was a railway worker, needed to, couldn't take care of her anymore, and needed someone else to take care of her and outside to work with her while she was alive and asked her questions and so forth. And then when she died in, I think, 1986 and performed histopathology on her brain afterwards to see what had happened to her brain. And that might be part of the disorder that he recognised or witnessed in her cognition. The first thing she said basically when she met him was that he had lost her so that his wasn't able to remember who she was, why she was there, etc.. You remember something? What is your name? Augusta. What's her last name? Augusta. Struggling to try and find the elements of her memory that she could bring to bear to the questions asked. But she wasn't capable, really, even when he saw her when she was 50. Of doing much in life anymore. And Alzheimer's disease is a very rapid progressive disease. On average is about eight years. This is increasing now as our treatments have improved, but on average about eight years from diagnosis to death. That's a very rapid. And for those who suffer a debilitating and disorienting disorder. One. One way of thinking about Alzheimer's disease is that it's a be like development in reverse. So, for example, if you think about how we develop over our normal life. And I think we can hold up ahead off the one, two, three months. We can speak a word after year. We can write sentences. After about 18 months, we can control and our defaecation and urine. After a couple of years, we can shower unaided. After four or five years, we can handle simple finances. After about ten years of age and hold it over for about 12. And if you look at the progression of the Alzheimer's disorders like this in reverse, you first lose your capacity to hold a job with the capacity to handle your finances, to dress yourself, to actually out of words, and finally to actually do most of the functions that we take for granted. So this is an idea that Alzheimer's disease is a bit like Retford genesis or development in reverse. So you probably have seen pictures like this before. This is a gross pathology of an Alzheimer's disease patient. On the left is a healthy brain. On the right is someone with advanced Alzheimer's. You can see the substantial thinning of the grey matter in the cortex as a large enlargement of the ventricles, in this case of very severe hippocampal damage, losing much of the hippocampus and therefore very much of our capacity for memory. On a microscopic level. This is what Alzheimer found 120 years ago as two major features that still remain prominent in our understanding of Alzheimer's disease. The first is that there are these things called senile plaques, which are these things about anywhere between about 1/10 and one half of a millimetre in size. Sometimes you can see them on unaided with the naked eye. And these are little disruptions of the brain structure, which are filled with a protein called amyloid beta. And on the right here is the other aspect of pathology that seems to be very prominent people with Alzheimer's disease. That's what's called neurofibrillary tangles. These are basically disordered conglomerations of a protein called Tao Utown, which has a large number of roles in regulating, especially transport of substances to the cell. And there's a lot they organise into long filaments and somehow they start that filaments start breaking down and start breaking into what we call tangles. So these Tao and Amyloid are the two major proteins associated with Alzheimer's disease. Yeah. Does it look like it? Absolutely. Yeah. It's one of the more fundamental proteins in ourselves. So now the question was, is Tao normally present or is it just present in in Alzheimer's disease brains? It is normally present. It's because of a particular. Way of how it interacts with other molecules of Tao. If this gets disordered, it starts to out of the solution and form these tangles. And so in Alzheimer's disease is proteins get slightly disordered. They get called hyper phosphorylated. Once they are hyper phosphorylated, they can come out of the solution form these tangles or neurofibrillary tangles within the cell. This slide here shows the kind of progression of these two different proteins, amyloid and TAL, as a function of time in the progression of Alzheimer's disease. It's hypothesised, it's not yet clear that there's not a huge amount of evidence for it. The amyloid first is causal. But certainly the case that amyloid emerges early on in dementia, followed by TAL and then after that followed by neurodegeneration loss of cells and sinuses, and after that followed by cognitive decline. The cognitive decline is probably paralleling the loss of self and finances, in fact, may even be emerging earlier. But the overt signs of cognitive decline happened late on in the season. The consequence of that means is that by the time you know that someone is suffering from dementia, from a cognitive perspective, it's already too late. Basically, our brain is very capable of making up things for ourselves and hiding the fact that it's losing its function. And by the time we find that someone is demented, that his pathological calmness, not to mention already too far gone to do much about it. We actually know surprisingly little about how amyloid and tal affect brain function. One of the reasons for this is it's actually been surprisingly difficult to generate animal models of Alzheimer's disease. It's not completely clear why this is. We can certainly generate animal models where we introduce mutated forms of amyloid or mutated forms of cow that have been mutations for which have been deduced from people who suffer from Alzheimer's disease. And we can see that those things can lead to the formation of plants and the formation of these neurofibrillary tangles. Often they do not lead to neurodegeneration and they only have mild effects on the cognitive performance of these animals. So why it is that it seems to be more the case in humans and so hard to generate these animal models of it is unclear. But the consequence of that is that we actually know precious little about the functional aspects of neurodegeneration. However, we do know a little bit. Whereas in the control condition here, these neurones are healthy of normal neuronal activity. This, by the way, is from a view from a Bush who is in the Dementia Research Institute. He's based. 100 metres away in the old building. No critical building site. Mark is a very prominent researcher in this field. We've also joined in the last little while. Be sorry when this app. This is a particular precursor to more don't worry too much about this is basically in animals who have this overproduction of amyloid beta and stand on these plaques. We often find hyperactivity increased activity in the brain. Later on when that android over production is joined by talent misfolding, we find a reduction in activity. It seems to be the case that this stage of the amyloid production stage. We can still rescue some of these phenotypes in mice, whereas once the towers joined the amyloid and started leading to hyperactivity, this phenotype seems to be incapable of being rescued. But it's still very early days to. So what do you mean by that? Is that you can rescue brain function. Yeah. Okay. Sorry. All right. Just use the word phenotype. And unfortunately, you can rescue brain function. That is, if you stop the production of amyloid, you can get cells back into the normal healthy state and stop degeneration. If you if you leave it later than that, you get to a stage where you can't seem to stop this process. In humans. We know quite a lot about the progression of Alzheimer's disease and the and the pathology that's associated with it. And in the large number of people that we see in the clinic, Alzheimer's disease is started in the temporal lobe, in the internal cortex or in the hip around the hippocampal formation. So starts this accumulation of talent and amyloid plaque starts down here and then starts to spread across the rest of the brain. It's a fairly slow process. Why is the temple open? I'm sorry. I just want to be see with my face and. Do have a of. Yeah, I think I believe the slide from this presentation, but I think is there in your notes which is an example where. PET imaging, positron emission tomography imaging, which can be used later to visualise the location of radio labelled proteins or substances we can introduce using PET. We can detect where this disordered amyloid or disordered pal is and we can therefore look at the location of those things in people. So. I think your next question would be why can we therefore say, is this person starting to suffer from dementia or something like that even before the cognitive symptoms emerge? That we haven't progressed to that stage yet. So we know that doing in people who are suffering from Alzheimer's, dementia, that you can see this structure and location of these disordered proteins. But these proteins are present in normal brains as well. And so in the early stages we can track it and we can kind of post-hoc reconstruct that this has happened. But in terms of being a specific predictor of what will happen in someone's brain. We're still not at that stage, if that makes sense. So we can see the presence of these proteins and the possibility that someone might be more likely to suffer from degeneration of dementia. We can't say on an individual specific basis that you will suffer from degeneration of dementia. It's not at that kind of specificity at the moment, but we can track those proteins at least with p t, which is very invasive. It's not something you can scale out to 150 million people in the world, but it that way. There is some hope, by the way, that and I haven't put it here, but we are starting at the moment. Other techniques. So, for example, the electroencephalogram, which, as I showed you, is very important at picking up seizures. Turns out there's very characteristic changes in that transfer ground at some stages of Alzheimer's disease as well. We'd like to think that maybe if we knew more about the structure of the circuits involved, also what's happening to the circuits involved? Early stages, we might be able to find signatures of that disorder even earlier on in patients and therefore be able, since EEG is a very cheap, easy thing to introduce, might be able to say have that in the clinic somewhere and just somewhat construe brief EEG study or something like that, four years age and see whether or not they're easy activities, predicting that they might be at risk of these diseases, whether we'll ever move to like proper predictive, I'm not sure. But it might be able to pick up some people from these methods. If you look at the stages of Alzheimer's disease and what you know of brain function, Hugo and I and others have told you over the last few weeks that the different stages of Alzheimer's disease make sense. If it starting in the temporal cortex, then the first stage is going to be hippocampal formation and final cortex. We know that these areas are important in spatial navigation, sexual orientation, and it's no surprise, therefore, to discover that one of the first things to go in Alzheimer's disease is your capacity to navigate around the world as the disorder moves up through the brain. This is just a schematic of that. For example, in the prior two cortex that you start to lose your visuospatial kind of organisation as we discuss the consequence of neglect. And as it moves more to say, the frontal cortex and you start to lose your abstract reasoning capacity. So the structure of the brain, a bit like those focal seizures, it is travelling wave on a much faster timescale, moving out across the brain. So this kind of travelling wave disorder from the temporal cortex to the rest of the brain has predictable cognitive consequences. I won't go through the news this time, but this is the kind of exam that you might do in the clinic to try and assess whether someone has dementia. I encourage you to do it and particularly to try and do it. The Where is it? Serial seventh task count back in seven for 100. I sometimes think I have to mention when I do this, I won't go through it now. What I want. And I also want to go through a couple of these hypotheses just in time. For a long time, we thought that some disruption to the cognitive system was important in the pathogenesis of dementia. It doesn't. Well, it might well be important for treating that. It doesn't seem to have much effect. Studies are being made trying to treat the cognitive system to see whether that has an impact on the occurrence of dementia. Very little progress has been made. By studying rare familial cases of dementia, that is, people who get early onset dementia. These tend to be much more genetic link and sporadic cases that occur later on in life. And there are some clear genes that are involved, most of which are related. Most of the known genes are related to some part of the amyloid pathway. And John Hardy, who is at the Institute of Neurology, is the one who's promoted the idea that amyloid is important in the pathogenesis of Alzheimer's disease. This is a slide, I think, from one of his reviews, but this is his basic hypothesis that there's some kind of change in how the brain metabolises and like beta, which in turn leads to all the other steps that cause degeneration and also the sort of power that we know happens later on. I won't go through these in any particular detail. The point is that this changes in the amyloid beta metabolism cause the the kinds of inflammation and injury to nerve cells that we see later on in the disorder and end up with dementia with plaque and have pathology. Now, consequence of this hypothesis is that if you were able to treat the amyloid beta early on, you might be able to stop the rest of the cascade and prevent or at least slow down degeneration. And many of you may be aware of this study from two weeks ago that came out among eight month study in collaboration with pharmaceutical companies looking at antibody against and why beta of people, family beta and immunising people with that antibody and seeing whether or not it might protect against Alzheimer's disease. And this is a figure from that paper which is cited down here and yet has a doesn't yet have an issue number. You can see that in placebo, there's no change in employee burden. There's when you immunise with this antibody, you get a substantial reduction in amyloid burden in the brain. The top one here. This shows the the dementia score. A person might take them to a clinic, but no one here says one of the performance measures for cognition. The yellow shows what's happening in patients who are being treated with this antibody, and the blue shows a placebo. And the fact that yellow line is above the blue line means an improvement in performance in those people who have taken the antibody over placebo. So this is, I think, something like a 20% improvement in cognition over this 18 month time period, which if you're excited, is a great improvement. If you're not excited, not much. It all depends which side you see on the fence there. I think even the hottest proponents here would say this is just the first step. Being able to try and treat Alzheimer's disease. It's certainly a long way to go. And it shows it is potentially, theoretically possible to try and stop Alzheimer's disease by blocking or interfering with this amyloid beta process early on. I'm aware that we need to shut up now. I just want to in the last couple of slides, just point out to you, we still don't know why some people suffer from dementia and others don't. What is it? Is there a is it just solely divide by genetics? Is it some kind of combination of genetics and environmental factors? What is actually the trigger for the start of degeneration in those people who have identical genes? There are several hypotheses about. I've listened to them in in this. At the end of this presentation you can go and read about them and the references there as well. The fundamental point at the end is that we don't know why some people are suffering from dementia and why others are not. It does seem, though, the most obvious thing to do for all of you and for me is that exercise, for whatever reason, seems to slow down dementia or prevent it. So I can only encourage you to walk more, run a bit, and exercise and have will also be here in about 50 years time. All right. Thanks for listening. Have a good week. Hope you get the. Still nine degrees. As a coach I'm wondering about. It's a good question. Should. That's a very good question. I don't know what's happening, but what I do is when I get back to October, I think, you know, I'm. I think in the limit you should be able to be done over at the zoo. But yeah. That's a good question because my. Sorry. What's that? You know what I'm saying? Yeah. You've got to let it go. Tell me nothing. Ever happened to you. Three. Yeah. Yeah. This is a component from the premise of migraine and. So from whatever, I think migraine itself is a spreading depression. So it's kind of the opposite in terms. Of so. The reduction in activity. I think it's. Kind of spreading the or the seasonality is pretty significant. So they're having different effects, but they're both spreading. They seem to have some link. And they're both chronic disorders of the normal people. So I don't know why they should be doing that. I think I just I know I, me, I oh, I'm like always like that sort of interesting that you look at the brain go away and it's like how like ultimately or mean like sort of like blind spots and then like, paralysis in the right hand spreading of the right side. Yeah. Yeah. Yeah. So and it's I mean, it is interesting. I know it's not to be treated as a patient over these things, but it is actually fundamentally interesting for. I remember seeing a. Presentation. A lot of people with migraine or or there's always structured and ways the leading edge of people often has these kind of it's clear answers. Which is the hope was that actually one should be able to correlate. Kinds of special circumstances getting on with the part of the visual cortex that was disrupted at the time. So it was I believe the early visual cortex had been disrupted. Can you see things like. Edges and stuff like that? Whereas. Like a bit of prudence. Maybe you'd see something, for example. I think that they hope to do that by. Because it's such a subjective experience. You have to be trying to ask people to map out what you see or represent. Mm hmm. Mm hmm. I don't know what is going to happen to. Not very strongly, but there was a couple. All right. Just a quick question to you. So do you get. Partial processes. Or do you mean that the neurones hiring, that's what's being asked you, don't you just just that because all of a sudden. Yeah, it's a really interesting point. You can kind of think of like this is one way of thinking. The brain is looking with a certain here. And then that. And so you get a little disruption here. The net effect of an isolated incident. Yes. And you were talking to each. Other over and over. I saw you. If you get disrupted this little circuit. I said. And then just to read this letter by something else. And that's how and how we keep. Says the student who was just here. Not it's not clear when that is on. At this stage, it doesn't seem like. It seems like. To start. Either that or the disruption of. To get something to. And. And if maybe it triggers. So that's one possibility. But this is too. Important for him to tell what causing. But I mean, I do think that. It's very hard and it have to bear in mind that these are within your own self. So. |